Reading Research Quarterly 44(3) pp. 218–253 • dx.doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.44.3.1 © 2009 International Reading Association
218
ABSTRACT
Reports from research and the larger educational community demonstrate that too many students have limited ability to
comprehend texts. The research reported here involved a two-year study in which standardized comprehension instruc-
tion for representations of two major approaches was designed and implemented. The effectiveness of the two experi-
mental comprehension instructional approaches (content and strategies) and a control approach were compared. Content
instruction focused student attention on the content of the text through open, meaning-based questions about the text.
In strategies instruction, students were taught specific procedures to guide their access to text during reading of the text.
Lessons for the control approach were developed using questions available in the teacher’s edition of the basal reading
program used in the participating classrooms. Student participants were all fifth graders in a low-performing urban district.
In addition to assessments of comprehension of lesson texts and an analysis of lesson discourse, three assessments were
developed to compare student ability to transfer knowledge gained. The results were consistent from Year 1 to Year 2. No
differences were seen on one measure of lesson-text comprehension, the sentence verification technique. However, for
narrative recall and expository learning probes, content students outperformed strategies students, and occasionally, the
basal control students outperformed strategies students. For one of the transfer assessments, there was a modest effect in
favor of the content students. Transcripts of the lessons were examined, and differences in amount of talk about the text
and length of student response also favored the content approach.
T
he pursuit of more precise understandings of
comprehension instruction has been an active,
ongoing area of research, at least since Dolores
Durkin’s (19781979) well-known criticism that very
little went on in classrooms that could be called com-
prehension instruction. The research reported here ad-
dresses the need for more precise understandings of
present-day comprehension instruction through the
implementation of standardized lessons on common
texts for two approaches to comprehension instruc-
tion, a strategies approach and a content approach, and
a comparison of their effects. The strategies approach
centers on the direct teaching of specific procedures,
such as summarizing, making inferences, and gener-
ating questions, and using them in working with text.
The other approach to comprehension, which we have
labeled a content approach, focuses on keeping students’
attention directed toward the content of what they are
reading and working through the text to build a repre-
sentation of the ideas through discussion.
Given that comprehension is such a complex cog-
nitive endeavor and is affected by, at least, the reader,
the text, and the context, comprehension research has
considered many features as contributing to student
outcomes. Here we will provide a glimpse into some
major areas of comprehension instructional research
as a way of illustrating the ancestry of the approaches
we are investigating. The framework that we will use
to provide this glimpse is the traditional perspective of
before, during, and after reading.
Considerations of what activities should happen be-
fore reading have centered on upgrading background
knowledge as a way to support students as they read.
Studies have examined background knowledge both in
terms of how it functions (Anderson & Pearson, 1984;
Johnson-Laird, 1983; Van Dijk & Kintsch, 1983) and
Rethinking Reading Comprehension
Instruction: A Comparison of
Instruction for Strategies and Content
Approaches
Margaret G. McKeown, Isabel L. Beck, Ronette G.K. Blake
University of Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, USA
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
219
how it can be upgraded. The majority of studies that
have examined effects of background knowledge has
demonstrated that upgrading can enhance students’
compre hen sion. T hi s re su lt h as b ee n fou nd w it h pr im ar y-
grade children (Beck, Omanson, & McKeown, 1982;
Pearson, Hansen, & Gordon, 1979), intermediate-grade
students (McKeown, Beck, Sinatra, & Loxterman,
1992), middle school students (Graves, Cooke, &
LaBerge, 1983), and high school students (Hood, 1981).
Two other prominent facets that have been pursued in
the before-reading arena are purpose for reading and
instruction of vocabulary to be encountered in an up-
coming selection. Research on providing students, or
asking students to develop, a purpose for reading is
sparse, but the studies that have been done show posi-
tive effects (Tierney & Cunningham, 1984). Despite the
scant amount of research, purpose for reading has be-
come an ingrained practice in conventional reading les-
sons. Teaching vocabulary can enhance comprehension
of text if the kind of instruction provided helps students
build meaningful associations to their knowledge base
and more than a brief definition is provided (Baumann,
Kame’enui, & Ash, 2003).
Before considering during-reading activities, which
are the focus of this paper, we will touch on after-
reading activities. After-reading activities often involve
one or another form of questioning. After Anderson
and Biddle (1975) reported that questions asked after
reading yielded better comprehension than merely re-
viewing the text did, attention turned to investigating
which kinds of questions were most effective. Examples
of questions singled out for being more effective include
those that ask about the most important text informa-
tion (Rickards, 1976), application questions (Rickards
& Hatcher, 1977–1978), and high-level questions (Yost,
Avila, & Vexler, 1977). Beck and McKeown (1981) de-
veloped a procedure to help teachers create questions
based on the most important information and the se-
quence of that information throughout the text.
Another avenue explored in developing effective
after-reading activities is interpretive discussion. Such
discussions typically focus on prompting students to
respond to a big questionthat arises from the text,
with an eye toward fostering critical-reflective think-
ing about text ideas (Wilkinson, Soter, & Murphy, in
press). Approaches to interpretive discussion that are
backed by evidence of success include Junior Great
Books (Great Books Foundation, 1987), Collaborative
Reasoning (Anderson, Chinn, Waggoner, & Nguyen,
1998), Philosophy for Children (Sharp, 1995), and
Grand Conversations (Eeds & Wells, 1989).
During-reading interventions emerged as efforts to
influence readers’ ongoing interactions with text. The
historical roots of during-reading interventions are
manifested in studies of inserted questions. Studies by
Watts and Anderson (1971) and Rothkopf (1966, 1972)
suggested that when students respond to questions
during reading, their understanding of the text is stron-
ger than it is if they simply read the text. Tierney and
Cunningham (1984), in their review of comprehension
instruction, suggested that deeper understanding of
during-reading questions was a worthy avenue to pur-
sue but that it needed to be tied to models of the text, of
the reader, or of mental processes.
Theoretical Foundations
and Current Status of Strategies
and Content Approaches
The development of the two approaches used in the re-
search reported herestrategies and content—came in
response to considering models of mental processing
as suggestions of ways to intervene for comprehension
development. Specifically, as is discussed in the next
section, the strategies approach developed from models
of thinking and learning processes and the content ap-
proach from models of text processing. A crucial impli-
cation of processing models is that learners need to be
mentally active to process text successfully. A common
feature of both the strategies and content approaches is
that they aim to engender active student engagement
with reading.
A major distinction between the two approaches is
that strategy instruction encourages students to think
about their mental processes and, on that basis, to ex-
ecute specific strategies with which to interact with
text. In contrast, content instruction attempts to engage
students in the process of attending to text ideas and
building a mental representation of the ideas, with no
direction to consider specific mental processes.
Strategies
The notion of providing instruction in strategies, rou-
tines for dealing with text, arose from work in devel-
opmental psychology that had established the active,
strategic nature of learning that developed as children
matured. Studies were then conducted that taught strat-
egies for general learning tasks. The strategies used in
such studies included rehearsal, categorization, and
elaboration (Brown, Bransford, Ferrara, & Campione,
1983).
Based on the developmental work, Brown and her
colleagues investigated the extent to which students
used various strategies for studying, such as note-
taking and underlining (Brown, 1981, 1982b; Brown
& Smiley, 1977). From their work, Brown and her col-
leagues surmised that it might be possible to improve
comprehension of young children or less-able learners
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
220
by teaching them effective study strategies (Brown &
Smiley, 1978). The eventual manifestation of this line of
work in relation to reading was reciprocal teaching, an
approach that taught young students to apply strategies
of summarizing, questioning, clarifying, and predicting
(Palincsar & Brown, 1984).
Strategies instruction also finds roots in models of
thinking. Symons, Snyder, Cariglia-Bull, and Pressley
(1989) traced notions of strategy teaching to the theo-
ries of Baron (1985) and Sternberg (1979, 1982), both of
whom emphasized that during the process of thinking
in problem-solving, competent thinkers employ strat-
egies such as identifying their goal, monitoring their
progress, and evaluating evidence. The reasoning that
followed was that providing young students with some
procedures they could employ while reading could fa-
cilitate their comprehension. These roots led Pressley et
al. (1992) to develop transactional strategies instruction,
an approach in which the teacher explains and models
strategies and uses these strategies to guide dialogue
about text.
In addition to reciprocal teaching (Palincsar &
Brown, 1984) and transactional strategies (Pressley et
al., 1992), programs of strategy instruction by Paris,
Cross, and Lipson (1984) and Duffy and Roehler (1989)
have also had a sustained impact on the area of strate-
gies instruction. Paris et al. focused their instructional
approach, informed strategies for learning, on develop-
ing awareness of the goals of reading and the value of
using strategies to pursue those goals. Their approach
is designed to teach students to evaluate, plan, and reg-
ulate as they build awareness of their processing. The
strategies taught in the informed strategies for learning
approach include understanding the purposes of read-
ing, activating background knowledge, allocating at-
tention to main ideas, evaluating critically, monitoring
comprehension, and drawing inferences. The work of
Duffy (1987) emphasized self-regulation and self-mon-
itoring, focusing on using strategies to remove block-
ages to comprehension. They emphasized the role of
direct explanation of strategies and the role of explicit
modeling in the instruction. Beyond these influential
programmatic approaches to strategies instruction, the
strategies literature comprises additional strategies pro-
grams (see, e.g., Anderson & Roit, 1993; Block, 1993;
Klingner, Vaughn, & Schumm, 1998) and numerous
smaller studies on a variety of strategies—implemented
both individually and in combination.
The most prominent review of the strategies lit-
erature was part of the National Reading Panel (NRP)
report (National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development [NICHD], 2000) that concluded that “the
past two decades of research appear to support the
enthusiastic advocacy of instruction of reading strate-
gies” (p. 4-46). The report identifies seven individual
strategies that the panel found to be supported by solid
evidence for improving comprehension: comprehension
monitoring, cooperative learning, graphic and semantic
organizers, question answering, question generation,
story structure, and summarization. The report sum-
marizes the effectiveness of the studies in each area,
providing a picture of overall success with using the
strategies.
Content
Although models of thinking and general learning
underlie strategies instruction, models developed to
explain specifically how a reader processes text (see,
e.g., Graesser, Singer, & Trabasso, 1994; Kintsch, 1974;
Trabasso, Secco, & van den Broek, 1984; van den Broek,
Young, Tzeng, & Linderholm, 1998) are the roots of a
content approach to comprehension. Text-processing
models take the perspective that the mental process-
es in reading focus on the development of coherence
based on organizing the meaningful elements of the
text. From a text-processing perspective, a reader moves
through text identifying each new piece of text informa-
tion and deciding how it relates to information already
given and to background knowledge (see Kintsch & van
Dijk, 1978). The focus is on what readers do with text
information to represent it and integrate it into a coher-
ent whole.
A text-processing perspective on comprehension
suggests that a content orientation may be a productive
direction for instructional intervention. That is, com-
prehension enhancement might derive from a focus on
continually striving for meaning as reading of the text
moves along rather than considering when and how to
call up specific routines to deal with new information. A
number of researchers have speculated that such an ap-
proach might be an alternative to direct strategies teach-
ing (Baker, 2002; Carver, 1987; Dole, Duffy, Roehler,
& Pearson, 1991; Gersten, Fuchs, Williams, & Baker,
2001; Kucan & Beck, 1997; Pearson & Fielding, 1991).
The body of research on content-focused approaches
is smaller than that of strategies approaches, and ques-
tioning the author (QtA; Beck & McKeown, 2006; Beck,
McKeown, Sandora, Kucan, & Worthy, 1996) may be
the approach that is most explicitly oriented toward a
text-processing view. The text-processing approach that
QtA connects with most directly is Kintschs (1998)
construction–integration model, in which there are two
phases: the construction phase, in which readers acti-
vate textual information, and the integration phase, in
which the activated ideas are integrated.
Other approaches that center on meaningful talk
about a text include in their orientation a sociocogni-
tive perspective, where the group is seen as forming an
interpretive community that jointly constructs mean-
ing. Such approaches generally fall under the label
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
221
collaborative discussion and typically initiate discussion
by focusing on a theme from the text or an issue-related
question, such as a question about a character’s motives.
These approaches include instructional conversations
(Saunders & Goldenberg, 1999), collaborative reason-
ing (Chinn, Anderson, & Waggoner, 2001), dialogic
instruction (Nystrand, 1997), and Junior Great Books
(Dennis & Moldof, 1983).
Summarizing approaches that focus on meaning-
ful talk about text, Applebee, Langer, Nystrand, and
Gamoran (2003) noted that although the approaches
have different vocabulary and routines, the form and
focus of the interventions significantly overlap, and
“results converge to suggest that comprehension of dif-
ficult text can be significantly enhanced by replacing
traditional I-R-E [Initiation-Response-Evaluation] pat-
terns of instruction with discussion-based activities”
(Applebee et al., 2003, p. 693). Other researchers have
found that discussion around text can promote problem-
solving, comprehension, and learning (Anderson et
al., 1998; Nystrand, 1997; Wegerif, Mercer, & Dawes,
1999). Discussion that leads to such outcomes fea-
tures open questions, student control of interpretive
authority, more student than teacher talk, and teacher
responses that are based on students’ responses (see,
e.g., Beck et al., 1996; Chinn, O’Donnell, & Jinks,
2000; Nystrand, Wu, Gamoran, Zeiser, & Long, 2003).
Thus, there appears to be a convergence of results that
discussion-based practices are effective for comprehen-
sion improvement, similar to the convergence of results
on the effectiveness of strategies instruction.
What We Still Don’t Know About Both
Approaches
An issue with both approaches is that there is still not
clear guidance on how to proceed instructionally. In
the case of strategies instruction, it seems that to make
instructional decisions we need to know which strate-
gies to use, how they should be taught, and how they
should be used in the course of reading. The available
research leaves all these factors in doubt. In terms of
which strategies are key, a large number of candidates
have been identified, with the National Reading Council
report (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998) and the NRP re-
port (NICHD, 2000) listing overlapping but different
sets of strategies that claim effectiveness. The more re-
cent work on strategies has advocated the instruction
of multiple strategies and the flexible coordination of
them. But which set of strategies should go into the mix
is not clear.
How strategies should be taught is not easily de-
rived from the research. One problem here is that what
goes on under a strategy label is not consistent from
study to study. In some cases, the same strategy label is
given to very different sets of activities. Our analysis of
the 18 studies highlighted in the NRP report (NICHD,
2000) as having shown positive results from instruc-
tion in summarizing showed a variety of tasks and
activities. For instance, in one summarization study,
students were taught steps for creating a summary, in-
cluding the following: (1) select main information, (2)
delete trivial information, and (3) relate to supporting
information (Rinehart, Stahl, & Erickson, 1986). In an-
other summarization study, students were asked direct
questions about the literal content of the text for the
purpose of leading students to draw an inference about
a characters actions (Carnine, Kame’enui, & Woolfson,
1982).
A similarly confounded picture emerges from ex-
amination of the studies labeled comprehension moni-
toring. A study in that category by Schmitt (1988)
instructed students in activating prior knowledge, set-
ting purposes, generating and answering prequestions,
forming hypotheses, verifying or rejecting hypotheses,
evaluating predictions, and summarizing. In contrast,
a study by Miller (1985) in the same category included
teaching students to ask themselves questions as they
read, such as “Is there anything wrong with the story?”
and to underline problems they found. Thus, not only
do activities under comprehension monitoring vary
widely but studies also include activities that are the
domain of another strategy, such as summarizing and
asking questions.
The foregoing discussion casts no aspersions on
the quality of the activities used within the studies but
rather points out that the sum total of studies leaves
us without a consistent picture of which strategies are
effective and what is effective about them. Typically, re-
search in content approaches to comprehension provide
only general directives on how students were brought
into discussion or how teachers learned to question and
to respond to students’ contributions in ways that were
productive toward building a coherent representation. It
may be that practice focusing on what is important and
making connections initiates readers’ mental engage-
ment with such strategies as summarization and infer-
ence, but they are not dealt with explicitly. Graesser
(2007) suggested that in the constructionintegration
model, “strategies exist but they do not drive the com-
prehension engine. Instead, the front seat of compre-
hension lies in the bottom-up activation(p. 11) of text
ideas and the integration of those ideas.
Outcomes measured in content approaches include
quality of discussion and comprehension of a story that
students had read and discussed and only rarely in-
vestigate subsequent achievement. For one content ap-
proach, QtA, although the focus of research has been
on changes in classroom discourse, three studies have
also included investigations of achievement (Beck et
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
222
al., 1996; Beck & McKeown, 1998; Sandora, Beck, &
McKeown, 1999). Sandora et al. compared the effects
on students’ comprehension of QtA and Junior Great
Books and found that students in the QtA approach had
greater recall and higher scores on answers to interpre-
tive questions than did those in the Great Books group.
The Beck et al. and Beck and McKeown studies included
an individually administered comprehension task on a
novel text passage that measured growth in comprehen-
sion monitoring and comprehension of the text. Both
the Beck et al. and Beck and McKeown studies showed
advantages for QtA students; in Beck et al., students im-
proved in monitoring, and in Beck and McKeown, both
monitoring and comprehension increased. However, the
results are limited. In the Beck et al. study, there was no
control groupstudents were given a pretest and post-
test. In Beck and McKeown, fifth and sixth graders were
compared with business-as-usual control students from
the same school. QtA fifth graders showed improvement
in monitoring, and sixth graders showed improvement
in both monitoring and comprehension. But because
of the transitory nature of the school population, there
were only 11 students in each approach in fifth grade
and 13 in each approach in sixth grade.
Thus, the findings about a content-oriented ap-
proach are limited by the number of studies, the small
sample size of those studies, and the sparse evidence
of improvement on tasks beyond the classroom dis-
cussions. This last characteristic is the opposite of a
drawback of the strategies findings. That is, for QtA
and discussion-based approaches, the results center on
interactions during discussion, whereas in strategies re-
search, there are few examples of what the classroom
interactions were like and thus little insight about what
led to the results.
Rationale for the Study
A limitation of the evidence for both strategies and
content approaches is that, with the exception of the
Sandora et al. (1999) study, the approaches have been
compared with only traditional instruction or unknown
instructionwhatever happened to be going on in
those other classrooms at the time. Little information
is available on what the instruction or texts were like
in the comparison groups. Additionally, strategies and
content approaches have never been compared with
each other.
Most importantly, studies of both types of ap-
proaches suffer from lack of standardization of what
teachers tell students, what students do, and how the
interactions proceed. In comparison, in research on
lower-level reading processes, the evidence for the role
of phonics was not simply that “phonics instruction is
good but that, to be good, instruction needed to be
systematic and sequential and include activities such as
blending (NICHD, 2000). Of course, teacher variation
plays a large role even with prepared materials, but in
the case of strategies and content approaches, the teach-
er is the only instructional agent. Thus, the research on
strategies and content approaches provides little guid-
ance on what in the instruction was responsible for the
outcomes. It could be the case that simply more time
and attention to text is the key that leads to improve-
ment (Carver, 1987), and the instructional activities and
prompts do not matter. We doubt that is the case; it is
more likely that some activities are more effective than
are others.
Overview of the Study
The research reported here involved a two-year study
in which we developed, implemented, and compared
standardized instruction for representations of two
major approaches targeted to enhance comprehension.
Student comprehension of common texts was examined
under the following approaches: strategies instruction,
content instruction, and basal instruction. Lessons were
designed around texts being used in the classrooms,
and thus students in all approaches dealt with the same
texts. These texts were the five core selections that
made up one theme within the basal reader in use in the
school. Each selection was the core text in a weekly les-
son plan that also included reading of another shorter
text and activities in fluency, writing, vocabulary, word
study, self-selected reading, and other typical language
arts practices.
The part of the five-day lesson plan labeled guided
comprehension consisted of reading the core text with
program-provided, scripted teacher questions. In this
study, we used the guided comprehension slice of the
weekly lesson plan to implement the lessons that we de-
veloped. Each lesson occupied a total of 45 to 75 minutes
a week within one of the daily 90-minute reading blocks.
During the other reading periods over the week, the
teachers engaged in whatever other activities they chose
from the basal reader, in accordance with their customary
classroom practice. Thus, this study was situated within
authentic contexts of classroom reading instruction.
In the first year of the study, this comprised lessons
for five narrative selections. For the second year of the
study, the same lessons for the five narrative selections
were implemented with a new cohort of students and
three expository text lessons were added. We hypothe-
sized that this longer period of instruction might make it
feasible to evaluate the effects of the instruction on com-
prehension of texts beyond those used in the classroom.
Thus a task for assessing transfer was developed.
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
223
The goal of this work is a greater understanding of
the extent to which instruction focused on content or
on strategies enhances students’ comprehension of spe-
cific texts and their independent comprehension abil-
ity. Findings from the study have the potential to yield
greater understanding because the use of standardized
lessons on common texts that represent both approach-
es allows a rather precise picture of what students do
during instruction and are able to do following instruc-
tion. Transcripts of videotaped lessons within the two
approaches afford opportunities to identify the kinds
of interactions that seem to be the most productive for
enhancing students’ abilities to deal with text ideas.
Investigation of comprehension of texts beyond those
used in instruction provides insight about the potential
long-term effects of the approaches.
Year 1
Method
The purpose of the study was to compare the effective-
ness of two experimental comprehension instructional
approaches and a control approach. One experimental
approach, the content approach, centered on readers’ de-
velopment of a coherent representation of the text. The
other experimental approach, the strategies approach,
involved direct teaching of explicit comprehension strat-
egies. The control approach, the basal approach, was
based on the instruction presented in the 2003 basal
reader that was used in the school district.
Materials
Lessons for each of the three approaches were devel-
oped based on 5 fifth-grade narrative text selections
from the basal reading program in use. The titles of the
five stories and a brief description of each story are pro-
vided in Appendix A.
Lesson Design
The format in each approach was teacher-directed,
whole-class instruction in which portions of the text
were read aloud, mostly by students, and were inter-
rupted with questions that elicited student discussion.
This is in keeping with customary guided comprehen-
sion provided in basal programs to the extent that ques-
tions are scripted for the teacher to deliver, and their
placement is designated within the reading selection.
The number of stopspoints at which reading was
stopped and discussion initiated—was similar for each
of the two experimental approaches. For the basal les-
sons, we used questions supplied in the teachers edi-
tion and their associated stopping points. Thus all three
approaches included questions interspersed within the
reading. The design of the questions for initiating and
guiding discussion in each approach is discussed in the
following sections. An example of a script for each ap-
proach is presented in Appendix B.
Content Approach
Content instruction focused student attention on the
content of the text through general, meaning-based
questions about the text. Reading was stopped and dis-
cussion initiated at purposely selected points when, for
example, a key character was introduced, some impor-
tant event had occurred, or where we judged some con-
fusion might arise for readers. This method is based on
the QtA approach (Beck & McKeown, 2006; Beck et al.,
1996). At each stopping point, the teacher provided an
initiating question to start the discussion. These open-
ended questions were designed to make public the im-
portant idea in a text segment (e.g., What’s going on
here?” “How does all this connect with what we read
earlier?”). Most stops also included a follow-up question
that the teacher could use to focus on the key ideas from
that portion of text. These follow-ups (e.g., “Can any-
one add to that?” “What does that mean?”) were used as
needed to elicit additional information.
Strategies Approach
In strategies instruction, students were taught to use
specific procedures to guide their access to text during
reading. An important notion behind teaching strate-
gies is that strategies instruction is productive because
it can be applied to any piece of text. At stopping points,
the teacher used a strategy to prompt discussion and
reminded students how to apply the strategy.
Strategies Selection
We chose the strategies to use for the present study af-
ter considering those highlighted in two recent reports
on reading, the National Reading Council report (Snow
et al., 1998) and the NRP report (NICHD, 2000). The
National Reading Council report focuses on the fol-
lowing strategies: summarizing, predicting, drawing
inferences, and monitoring for coherence and misun-
derstandings. The NRP report lists seven individual
strategies for which there was evidence of improved
reading comprehension after instruction: comprehen-
sion monitoring, summarization, question generation,
question answering, cooperative learning, story struc-
ture, and graphic and semantic organizers.
In selecting among recommended strategies, we
considered which procedures might most naturally be
called on as a reader works through a text to understand
the content and also which strategies were most appro-
priate for a discussion-based lesson. Our thinking was
that readers may summarize important information as
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
224
they move through text, develop a sense of what may
be coming next, draw inferences to create connec-
tions, and form questions to check that they are on
track. Additionally, effective readers monitor their un-
derstanding. We therefore selected summarizing, pre-
dicting, drawing inferences, question generation, and
comprehension monitoring for strategy-based lessons.
The strategy to be used at each stopping point was
selected depending on which strategy fit best at that
point in the text (e.g., if the story reached a turning
point, we would ask the students to predict, or if the
story presented confusing information, we asked the
students to check their understanding).
Expert Review
Because our expertise is in content instruction, to de-
velop the strategy lessons, an intensive process of les-
son review and revision was undertaken. We designed
a three-stage review process that elicited feedback from
strategies experts on how well our strategy lessons rep-
resented strategies instruction as understood within the
reading research community and from teachers on is-
sues of classroom implementation.
The first review cycle involved three strategies ex-
perts from the national research community. This cycle
was designed to elicit extensive open-ended comments
on strategy selection and early drafts of the lessons.
Based on the comments, lessons were revised. The sec-
ond cycle involved 10 reading specialists/coaches. This
stage was designed to elicit reading practitioners’ evalu-
ation of ease of lesson use. The third cycle involved six
additional nationally recognized strategies experts. This
stage was designed to elicit more structured responses
from a larger group of strategies researchers on our most
complete version of the lessons.
Thus, in the first stage of review, the experts pro-
vided comments on our initial strategy lessons in terms
of usability and fidelity to strategies instruction. In the
second and third stages, reviewers were asked to rate
strategies instruction in the lessons on the following six
dimensions, as well as to provide written comments:
1. Coherent, student-friendly language
2. Adequate representation of the strategy
3. Teachers’ ability to instruct the strategy
4. Students’ ability to follow teacher directives
5. Adequate practice using the strategy
6. Appropriate student scaffolding toward
independent strategy use
Reviewers were asked to rate, on a 4-point scale,
how well each of the five implemented strategies (e.g.,
summarize) was operationalized across each of the six
dimensions, providing 30 ratings from each reviewer.
The scale was as follows: 4 = almost always, 3 = usu-
ally, 2 = sometimes, 1 = does not occur. The average
rating from the reading practitioners across the six di-
mensions for the five strategies (second stage of review)
was 3.73. The six nationally recognized strategies ex-
perts (third stage of review) were more critical (as was
expected). Their ratings averaged 3.26. See Table 1 for
means and standard deviations of the ratings from read-
ing practitioners and national experts for each of the
six dimensions. The experts provided specific feedback
about the lesson, noting that if their suggestions were
incorporated their ratings would be higher. Lessons
were revised accordingly.
Basal Approach
For the lessons in the basal approach, we developed
scripts based on the questions in the teacher’s edition
of the basal reading program used in the participating
classrooms. Because so much material is presented in
the teacher’s edition, we included only questions that
were presented for use during reading of the selection
Dimension
Reading practitioners
M (SD)
Strategy experts
M (SD)
Coherent language 3.65 (0.41) 3.80 (0.40)
Adequate strategy representation 3.85 (0.35) 3.37 (0.69)
Teachers’ ability to instruct 3.92 (0.21) 3.13 (0.79)
Students’ ability to follow directives 3.60 (0.41) 3.17 (0.67)
Adequate strategy practice 3.73 (0.30) 3.25 (0.81)
Appropriate scaffolding 3.63 (0.52) 2.83 (0.58)
Table 1. Practitioner and Expert Ratings of Strategy Lessons by Dimension on a 4-Point Scale
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
225
and limited the questions to those directed to compre-
hension. For example, we omitted questions about word
recognition, grammar, and so on, but asked the ques-
tions about text indicated at the stopping points in the
teachers edition. Thus the approach we created was not
a business-as-usual control and thus was more accu-
rately called basal comprehension because we may have
provided a stronger focus on comprehension during the
reading of the basals major weekly text.
Participants
Students
Student participants were all fifth graders from six intact
classrooms who took reading in their regular classroom
in one school in a small, urban district in southwestern
Pennsylvania. The district in which the study took place
had been under court-ordered desegregation from 1981
until 2000. In 2004–2005, the school was identified as
in need of improvement” by the Pennsylvania System
of School Assessment, with 48% of the fifth graders
scoring at basic or below basic in reading according to
the 2005 assessment. A majority of the students were
African American (58%) and about half of the students
(49%) qualified for free or reduced-price lunch.
To the best of our knowledge, the students’ reading ex-
periences had been through commercial basal programs.
We base this on knowledge of the curriculum in the dis-
trict, mobility patterns in the district, and the curriculum
in adjoining districts. First, there was mobility among the
student population within the district, but the same basal
program was used throughout the district. Second, there
was some mobility with two adjoining districts, a large ur-
ban district and a small, high-poverty urban district. Both
of these districts also had basal programs in place.
Assignment to Approach
Three sets of test results were used to determine com-
parability of the six classrooms and to assign two class-
rooms to each of the three instructional approaches.
The first were the Terra Nova reading comprehension
(McGraw-Hill, 2000) scores for 132 students who had
taken the test in the spring of their fourth-grade year
and were expected to enter fifth grade in the fall. After
we removed from the study pool students who trans-
ferred out of the school before fifth grade began and
special education students who did not take reading
with their regular class and added 13 students who
transferred into the school to begin fifth grade, we had
a final sample size of 119 students, with Terra Nova
reading comprehension scores for 105 of these stu-
dents. We then administered two Woodcock-Johnson
(Woodcock, Mather, & Schrank, 2004) decoding tests
(Word Identification and Word Attack) to these 119
students.
To evaluate whether there were significant differ-
ences among the classes in reading ability, one-way
analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were done for the two
Woodcock-Johnson tests and the Terra Nova test. There
was not a significant difference among the six classes for
the Word Identification test, F(5, 111) = 1.39, p = .234,
or for the Word Attack test, F(5, 111) = 1.64, p = .156.
For the Terra Nova scores, the ANOVA indicated that
classes were not equivalent, F(5, 100) = 2.64, p < .03.
Post-hoc tests revealed that one classroom’s scores dif-
fered from two other classrooms at a p = .08 level of
significance. See Table 2 for the pretest scores for all
individual measures.
To assign the six classes (and the corresponding
teachers) to the three instructional approaches and
to make them as comparable as possible, we ranked
Approach Class #
Word Identification test
M (SD)
Word Attack test
M (SD)
Reading comprehension
M (SD)
Standard score
M (SD)
Content 1 4.95 (1.33) 5.99 (3.40) 8.84 (8.13)
2 5.08 (1.67) 5.54 (3.87) 5.40 (3.61)
Overall 5.02 (1.51) 5.75 (3.63) 6.73 (5.93) 0.25 (2.70)
Strategies 3 6.13 (2.71) 8.04 (5.25) 9.40 (5.70)
4 4.72 (1.53) 5.41 (3.95) 6.05 (3.01)
Overall 5.38 (2.25) 6.65 (4.73) 7.68 (4.76) 0.39 (2.64)
Basal comprehension 5 5.60 (1.93) 6.96 (4.79) 6.85 (3.15)
6 5.04 (1.91) 4.58 (2.94) 5.07 (2.28)
Overall 5.33 (1.91) 5.81 (4.12) 5.96 (2.85) 0.10 (2.57)
Table 2. Mean (SD) Scores for Three Baseline Assessments and Overall Standard Scores of Reading Ability for Each
Approach for Year 1
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
226
the classrooms from highest (Classroom 1) to lowest
(Classroom 6) according to the mean classroom scores
on the Terra Nova and Woodcock-Johnson tests. We
also observed the classrooms to get a sense of class-
room dynamic (e.g., student behavior, teacher discipline
style), and these factors were considered when yoking
the classrooms. In particular, when the classes were
close on the other criteria, we took into account class-
room dynamic, especially student behavior. Given that
three classrooms exhibited a relatively high number of
student disruptions, we included one of these class-
rooms in each approach.
To test for comparability across the three pairs of
classrooms for the two Woodcock-Johnson subtests and
the Terra Nova assessment, we combined the three scores
into one standard score by converting each individual
score from each of the three tests into a z score and then
summing the three z scores. After a standard score was
computed for each student, an ANOVA was conducted.
Standard scores by approach are shown in Table 2 on
page 224. No significant differences among the three ap-
proaches were found, F(2, 116) = 0.620, p = .540.
Teachers
All six fifth-grade classroom teachers and three sup-
port teachers were invited to participate in the study
and all agreed to do so. The nine teachers each received
a $300 stipend for their participation, which included
a half-day initial training, three after-school meetings,
preparation time for conducting the lessons, e-mail cor-
respondence with research staff, and exit interviews.
Each of the classroom teachers was assigned to ap-
proach by virtue of the class he or she taught, per the
process described above. Each pair of classes (and the
corresponding teachers) was randomly assigned to one
of the three approaches. The three supporting teachers
(two Title I teachers and one special education inclusion
teacher) who would conduct make-up lessons were also
assigned to approaches. One teacher, the special educa-
tion inclusion teacher, had already been assigned to one
of the classrooms by the school district so she had to
be kept with that classroom and the other in the pair.
The other two support teachers were not previously as-
signed, so they were randomly assigned to the remain-
ing two approaches.
Eight teachers were women (one was a man); all were
European American. The number of years of teaching
experience ranged from 2 years to 23 years. Specifically,
the two teachers in the content approach had 2 years’
and 18 years’ experience; the two teachers in the strate-
gies approach had 15 years’ and 23 years’ experience,
and the two teachers in the basal-comprehension ap-
proach had 5 years’ and 21 years’ experience. All teach-
ers had spent their entire teaching careers in the district
in which the study occurred.
As mentioned, a basal reading program was in use
in the district and basal readers had been the core of
the reading curriculum for decades. However, to get a
sense of how the teachers implemented the guided com-
prehension portion of the weekly reading lesson, two
members of the research staff observed a guided read-
ing lesson for each teacher. The observations focused
on the kinds of questions teachers asked, the extent to
which they engaged students in discussion, and how
they dealt with student responses. The observations in-
dicated the following:
Five of the six teachers used predominately ques-
tions from the basal. The sixth teacher did not
use the basal questions but asked questions that
elicited student experiences and inserted a good
many of his own experiences that were tangen-
tially related to the text (this teacher was assigned
to the content approach).
Four of the five teachers that used basal questions
conducted the lesson mainly through an Initiate-
Respond-Evaluate pattern. That is, they posed the
basal question; accepted a brief, mostly literal, re-
sponse from a student; and went on.
The fifth basal questioning teacher frequently
followed up student responses by elaborating on
them or probing students for more information
(this teacher was assigned to the basal approach).
The points above indicated that in four of the six
classrooms, there was essentially no discussion. In the
classroom in which the teacher focused on students’ ex-
periences, the discourse was a collection of student ex-
periences but was not a discussion, in that there was no
building of relationships among student comments. In
the remaining classroom, the teacher’s follow-up of stu-
dent responses provided some threads of connection,
and thus the discourse had elements of a discussion, in
contrast to the Initiate-Respond-Evaluate pattern.
Measures
We used a variety of measures to assess the outcomes
of the three approaches. There were two categories of
assessments: lesson-text comprehension and beyond-
lesson-text-assessments. The lesson-text comprehen-
sion assessments included the sentence verification
technique (SVT; Royer, Hastings, & Hook, 1979) for
each text of the five lessons implemented in this study
and included story recall for two of those lesson texts.
We selected the two lesson-comprehension measures to
represent different levels of comprehension. The SVT
requires recognition of text content, and recall requires
recalling content from memory and constructing lan-
guage to express it. We also analyzed transcripts of
classroom discussions from two of the five lessons to
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
227
consider differences in discourse during the lessons
among the approaches. The beyond-lesson assessments
included a comprehension-monitoring task and a task
that assessed knowledge of strategies.
Lesson-Text Comprehension
SVT
The SVT was developed and used extensively by Royer
and his colleagues (1979). Because of its extensive track
record and because it is a measure that can be devel-
oped for specific texts, it fit our purposes well. In this
task, students examine sentences and determine wheth-
er each is true about the text (Royer, Carlo, Dufresne, &
Mestre, 1996).
Task Development. In the SVT version we de-
veloped, the items were either paraphrases (PPs) or
inferences of ideas from the text. This format was de-
veloped because theory suggests that performance on
paraphrased items represents a more text-bound level
of comprehension, whereas performance on inference
items represents comprehension of gist. For each item, a
true and false (PP), local inference (LI), and global infer-
ence (GI) of an idea from the text were designed. An LI
required students to draw an inference about a localized
portion of the text, whereas a GI required students to
make a connection across a large portion of the text.
Because the stories were of different lengths, the
number of items across the tests varied, ranging from
36 items to 60 items. To exemplify the types of items
on our SVTs, we created items for a well-known story,
The Three Little Pigs, and those items are presented in
Appendix C.
Pilot Work. We conducted two phases of pilot work
on the SVTs. The first involved students from a different
school in the same district in which the present study
was implemented. We administered the assessment to
a group of students who read one of the stories with-
in a strategies lesson and a group who did so within a
content lesson. There were 14 students in each group.
Additionally, we piloted the SVT items for all five texts
with students from a similar demographic who were at-
tending a summer school program. We divided the 40
students into two groups and administered the strat-
egies lessons to one group and the content lessons to
the other. With information from the pilot results, we
revised the measures, removing floor and ceiling items
and adding new items as necessary.
Administration. For three of the lessons, the SVT
was administered immediately following the reading
lesson. For the two stories for which oral recalls were
taken, the SVT was administered after oral recalls were
collected. The SVTs were administered to an entire class
by a research team member who read each item aloud.
Scoring. Each SVT was scored for total items cor-
rect, GIs correct, LIs correct, and PPs correct. Overall,
SVT comprehension scores of the texts were calculated
by averaging the scores from the five stories, and this
was done for each scoring dimension. This yielded four
scores, one for each item type (GI, LI, PP) and a total
score. Internal consistency of the total scores for the five
narrative SVTs, as assessed by Cronbach’s Alpha, was
equal to .901.
Oral Recall
Oral recalls were collected from all students for two
of the five stories used in the lessons, Off and Running
(Soto, 2005) and The Fun They Had (Asimov, 2005).
Administration. After each class had completed the
lesson for the two recall-targeted stories, research team
members took recalls from each individual student.
The recall protocol involved four prompts: one initiat-
ing prompt and three additional prompts to encourage
students to recall as much as possible. Recalls were re-
corded and subsequently transcribed.
Scoring. Recalls were scored for length and qual-
ity. Scoring for both dimensions involved dividing
the story texts and student recalls into content units.
Content units are units of text about one clause in
length (Omanson, 1982), a unit length we have used in
previous work (Beck et al., 1982). The number of con-
tent units in each student recall that represented a text
content unit was used as the length measure, and the
level of importance of each of these units was the basis
for the quality measure.
Importance was determined by categorizing each
unit as major, support, or detail. A major unit was de-
fined as containing ideas that are very important to the
story and a key component of a good summary. A sup-
porting unit, although not necessarily key, holds main
ideas together and adds elaborative information to the
major units. A summary could do without these ideas,
but it would be less complete. A detail unit was defined
as less-essential information. These ideas do not hold
the story together, and a summary would be complete
without them.
Off and Running was divided into 282 content units,
with 17 major ideas, 36 supporting ideas, and the rest
considered detail units. The Fun They Had was divided
into 182 content units, with 12 major ideas, 21 sup-
porting ideas, and the rest detail units. The units were
defined and labeled (as major, support, or detail) by one
researcher, after which all researchers discussed the cat-
egorization and reached consensus.
Prior to scoring, the recalls were blinded as to in-
structional approach. After blinding, each student recall
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
228
was divided into content units, and those that men-
tioned a text content unit were counted and used as the
length score.
For quality scoring, points were assigned to each
unit: major units were assigned a value of three points,
supporting units two points, and details one point.
Recalls were scored by one team member and 20%
were rescored by a second team member to determine
inter-rater reliability. Differences for the 20% were then
resolved through consensus. For Off and Running, 88%
agreement was reached, and for The Fun They Had, 86%
agreement was reached.
As was done for the SVTs, an overall comprehension
score was calculated for each student for the recalls.
This score was calculated by averaging the recall scores
from the two stories.
Pilot Work. To get a sense of how student recalls
would align with the text content units, we collected
oral recalls for Off and Running and The Fun They Had
from the same group of summer school students with
whom we had piloted the SVTs. The information gath-
ered contributed to the development of rules for scoring
students’ wording that did not match text wording but
represented text ideas.
Lesson Discourse
As a way to understand the role of discussion during
the lessons in promoting comprehension, we exam-
ined transcripts of the lessons on the texts for which
students had provided recalls. We viewed promoting
comprehension as the extent to which the discussion
focused on building meaning from the text and used
two metrics to capture this. One was the proportion of
the discussion that was text based, which was instanti-
ated as talk that matched any of the content units, and
the other was the length of student responses, which
was instantiated as number of words per student re-
sponse. Proportion of text-based talk seemed a direct
measure of focus on the text. Regarding length of stu-
dent response, we have noted in previous research that
as discussion becomes more meaning focused, students’
responses become longer (Beck et al., 1996; McKeown,
Beck, & Sandora, 1996).
Beyond-Lesson-Text Assessments
Comprehension Monitoring
To explore whether students’ awareness of their com-
prehension (i.e., comprehension monitoring) was differ-
ent across the three instructional approaches, we used
a task based on one we have used previously in which
students read a short text containing some inconsisten-
cies, were stopped at several intervals, and asked wheth-
er there was anything that did not make sense (Beck
et al., 1996). The rationale for this comprehension-
monitoring task is that the ability to make decisions
about whether comprehension is proceeding success-
fully would seem to be a precursor to being able to take
steps to address problems. We assessed whether there
was any change in students’ comprehension monitoring
through a pre- and posttest on nonlesson stories that
were counterbalanced.
Task Development. Two 473-word stories with
a Flesch-Kincaid 3.3 grade-level readability (Flesch,
n.d.) were developed. Each had five paragraphs, with
three of the paragraphs including one inconsistency,
one paragraph including two inconsistencies, and one
paragraph with no inconsistencies. The stories were
composed in a familiar, fable-like style using student-
friendly language.
Administration. The comprehension-monitoring
task was individually administered, audiotaped, and
subsequently transcribed. The researcher read the text
aloud as the student followed along, a format that was
used to prevent decoding from confounding compre-
hension. The researcher stopped after each paragraph
and asked whether anything did not make sense. If the
students did not detect an error in the paragraph, the
researcher prompted them further by rereading two ad-
jacent sentences, one of which contained an error for
the four paragraphs that had errors, and asking the stu-
dent to explain those sentences.
Scoring. The comprehension-monitoring measure
was scored on a 2-point scale for each paragraph. In the
error paragraphs, students were given two points if they
detected the target error without additional prompt-
ing and one point if they detected the error only after
the researcher reread the sentences in which the error
existed. For the error-free paragraphs, two points were
awarded if students recognized there were no errors
and also were able to explain two important target sen-
tences. A second rater scored 20% of the responses and
an inter-rater reliability of 95% was obtained.
Strategies Task
The strategies approach provided intensive instruction
and practice during which each strategy was explicitly
named, described, and discussed. Given this instruc-
tion, we might expect the students in the strategies ap-
proach to show greater knowledge of strategy terms and
ability to apply them to text. Yet, strategy terms are also
introduced in basal reading lessons, including the basal
series used in our study, and from our interactions with
school personnel, we learned that the fifth-grade stu-
dents had been exposed to this instruction. Thus we
wondered whether knowledge of strategies would in-
deed differ across approaches.
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
229
To compare strategies knowledge across approaches,
we developed an additional task that was administered
as a posttest. The task was designed to measure each
student’s knowledge of strategies used in this study. The
task was based on one developed by Kozminsky and
Kozminsky (2001).
Task Development. Six stories were developed, av-
eraging 349 words, with an average 3.6 Flesch-Kincaid
grade level (Flesch, n.d.). For each story, a correspond-
ing four-question multiple-choice test was developed.
The questions covered four of the five strategies taught
in the strategies approach: asking a question about im-
portant information, predicting, inferring, and summa-
rizing. We did not include comprehension monitoring
because it did not lend itself to multiple-choice questions
(and it was assessed with a separate comprehension-
monitoring test, as described above).
The test questions were identical for all stories and
presented in the same order:
1. Which question might a teacher ask to know if a
student had understood the important ideas from
the story?
2. What would be a good prediction about what
might happen next in the story?
3. Reading between the lines, what could you infer
from this story?
4. Which of these is a good summary of the story?
Administration. All six strategy-task stories were ad-
ministered, two per classroom, in the first year of the
study. Four different stories were tested per approach,
so that every story was administered in two classrooms,
each in a different approach of the study. The task was
administered to the whole class. To prevent decoding
issues from confounding students’ strategy knowledge,
the stories, the four questions, and four choices were
read aloud.
Scoring. Scoring was straightforward. Students were
given one point for each correct answer, with a possible
total of eight points across two tests.
Procedures
Teacher Training
Prior to implementation of the study approaches, the
research team provided training to teachers. Training
began with a short overview of the study’s purpose.
Then teachers for each approach met separately for a
half day to receive intensive training in their assigned
approachcontent, strategies, or basal comprehension.
Teachers received a notebook containing all lesson ma-
terials for their approach. Finally, the scripted lessons
were reviewed and discussed. Teachers were asked not
to share their lessons and materials with teachers in the
other approaches, and we found no indication that they
had done so.
One week after training, each participant group ob-
served a demonstration lesson of their approach by one
researcher in one of their fifth-grade classrooms. The
teachers had a copy of the scripted lesson and an ob-
servation sheet that prompted them to attend to certain
features of the instruction. A question-and-answer ses-
sion followed.
Student Preparation
Students were prepared for the two experimental ap-
proaches through practice lessons developed by the
research team and delivered by the teachers. Student
preparation for the strategies approach consisted of
three 45-minute lessons over three days. Each of the
five strategies targeted for the study was introduced and
practiced first with several sentences and then with a
short piece of text. Two strategies were introduced and
practiced in the first lesson and two more in the second
lesson. For the final lesson, the last strategy was intro-
duced and all five strategies were practiced. Students
were given a reminder sheet that briefly described each
strategy. Students were told to use these sheets during
the preparation lessons as well as in their classrooms
when the five story lessons were implemented.
Student preparation for the content approach was
done in one session in which the teacher told students
that during some upcoming reading lessons they would
be going through a story together, reading a portion of
the text, and stopping to talk about what they had read.
They then practiced doing this on a short text. There was
no preparation for the basal-comprehension approach
because the lessons were based on the comprehension
questions available in the guided comprehension portion
in the teacher’s edition of the basal reader being used.
Implementation of Lessons
and Assessments
The three instructional approaches were implemented in
the six fifth-grade classrooms within the selected school,
with two classes per approach. The instruction began in
the middle of January, and over the next five weeks, the
teacher in each classroom taught a story lesson at the
beginning of each week. Every lesson was observed by
a researcher. During observations, the researcher com-
pared the script of the lesson to the implementation in
the classroom. Feedback was only called for after the ini-
tial lesson, in which one teacher in the content approach
and one teacher in the strategies approach made minor
deviations from the script. During all subsequent obser-
vations, we judged the lessons to be implemented with a
high degree of fidelity for all teachers.
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
230
To check fidelity, the transcripts of two subsequent
lessons, the second and the fifth, were analyzed for the
percentage of scripted initial questions asked, the extent
to which the questions elicited an appropriate response,
and whether useful follow-up questions were asked.
The analyses indicated that teachers implemented the
lessons with a high degree of fidelity.
For both stories across all three approaches, the ini-
tial scripted question was asked 100% of the time and
elicited an appropriate response from a student 89% of
the time in the content approach and 100% of the time
in the strategies and basal-comprehension approaches.
Teachers posed a follow-up question when needed,
which were scored appropriate 94% of the time for the
content approach and 100% of the time for the strate-
gies and basal-comprehension approaches. The results
were virtually the same for the fifth story. These very
high scores are not surprising given the scripted nature
of the lessons.
Two of the stories were completed within one class-
room period of 45 minutes; the other three stories were
divided into two parts that were delivered on consecu-
tive days. All lessons were audiotaped for later tran-
scription. Two of the lessons were also videotaped. At
the completion of three of the lessons (lessons 1, 3, and
4) all students were administered the SVT by a research
team member. After the other two lessons (lessons 2
and 5), students were first individually administered
oral recalls, which were audiotaped for subsequent
transcription. When the oral recalls were completed for
a class, the students were administered the SVT.
After all story lessons and their corresponding as-
sessments were completed, the strategies test was admin-
istered to each classroom by a research team member.
Finally, students were individually administered the
posttest of the comprehension-monitoring task.
Results
Questions of interest in the first year included two ques-
tions addressed to lesson comprehension:
1. Were there differences in comprehension among
approaches for lesson texts?
2. Did patterns in the lesson discourse help to ex-
plain any differences found?
Two other questions addressed more general outcomes:
3. Were there differences in students ability to mon-
itor comprehension on nonlesson texts among
approaches?
4. Were there differences in students’ knowledge of
comprehension strategies among approaches?
Using the student as the unit of analysis, data were
analyzed with one-way ANOVAs (analyses of covari-
ance were also run using the calculated standard score
of reading ability as a covariate; all findings were con-
sistent with the ANOVA results), with Tukey’s honestly
significant difference (HSD) tests run for post-hoc dif-
ferences (we examined our data for classroom effects
with a nested analysis, and there were no significant
effects; in considering this, we decided to maintain our
one-way ANOVA because our power would be drasti-
cally reduced due to the small number of classrooms in
our study if we analyzed our data using a nested analy-
sis). The exception was the comprehension-monitoring
pre- and post measure, which was analyzed with a re-
peated measures ANOVA with approach (content, strat-
egies, basal comprehension) as the between-subjects
variable and test time (pre or post) as the within-sub-
jects variable. Effect sizes are reported using a Cohen’s d
standardized effect size, which is calculated as the dif-
ference in group means divided by the pooled standard
deviation.
Lesson-Text Comprehension Measures
Two comprehension assessments were used for lesson
texts: SVT and story recall. The SVT was administered
after each of the five lessons. Story recalls were collected
after two of the lessons. Scores for these assessments
were calculated by averaging scores across stories.
SVT
The SVT data for each of the five stories were analyzed
for differences in four areas: total score, GIs correct, LIs
correct, and PPs correct. Analyses showed that there
were no significant differences across approaches for any
of the four areas. The results are presented in Table 3.
As can be seen from the table, all three groups
did moderately well on the SVT tests, with a mean
high of 81.20% correct and a low of 76.94% correct.
Additionally, 98% of the scores were above 50%, and
about half of the scores were above 80%.
Recall
The oral recalls collected for two of the five stories were
analyzed on two dimensions: length of recall and qual-
ity of recall. The results of these analyses are presented
in Table 4.
As indicated in the table, the approaches had sig-
nificantly different recall lengths and quality scores.
Post-hoc tests (Tukey’s HSD) on these results indicated
that the recalls of the content group (M = 16.20) were
significantly longer than were those of the strategies
group (M = 10.33; p = .001). The length of the basal-
comprehension students’ recalls (M = 14.13) was also
significantly higher than was that of the strategies
group (p = .05). The length of the basal-comprehension
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
231
students recalls was not significantly different from that
of the content students’ recalls (p = .373; d = .270).
Analysis of data for quality of the oral recalls, pre-
sented in Table 4, revealed that the scores of the ap-
proaches also differed significantly on this dimension.
Post-hoc analyses indicated that content students
(M = 33.40) produced higher quality recalls than did
the strategies students (M = 22.97; p = .001). The quality
score of the basal-comprehension students (M = 29.20)
was not significantly different from the average scores of
the content group (p = .292; d = .310) or the strategies
group (p = .09; d = .627).
Lesson Discourse
The analysis of lesson transcripts from the two lessons
from which we had recalls involved examining two
patterns. The first was the proportion of talk that direct-
ly reflected text ideas. The two lessons were combined,
and the number of text-based words was tallied at each
stop. The second was the amount of talk in each student
turn during the discussion. Differences were analyzed
separately for student and teacher talk.
As seen in Table 5, the ANOVA for text-related stu-
dent talk revealed a significant difference. Post-hoc tests
(Tukeys HSD) indicated that content lessons had a sig-
nificantly higher proportion of text-based student com-
ments (94%) than did strategies lessons (75%; p = .005)
and that the basal-comprehension lessons also had a
significantly higher proportion of text-based comments
(86%) than did the strategies lessons (p = .002). The
content and basal-comprehension lessons were not sig-
nificantly different from each other (p = .209; d = .463).
Content
(n = 44)
Strategies
(n = 37)
Basal comprehension
(n = 38)
Score type M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F p d
a
Total % 78.30 (9.75) 78.42 (9.07) 79.41 (9.05) 0.168 .846 .107
Global inferences % 76.94 (9.63) 77.03 (8.11) 78.13 (8.67) 0.281 .804 .139
Local inferences % 78.52 (9.69) 78.83 (10.02) 78.61 (10.20) 0.010 .990 .026
Paraphrases % 79.17 (11.42) 79.22 (10.40) 81.20 (9.44) 0.474 .624 .181
Table 3. Mean (SD) Scores and Analysis of Variance Results for Percent of Sentence Verification Technique Items Correct
for Narrative Texts by Approach for Year 1
a
d = Cohen’s d standardized effect size, calculated as the difference in group means divided by the pooled standard deviation.
Content
(n = 44)
Strategies
(n = 37)
Basal comprehension
(n = 38)
Score type M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F p
Length 16.20 (8.96) 10.33 (4.55) 14.13 (6.14) 7. 29 .001
Quality 33.40 (16.14) 22.97 (9.61) 29.20 (10.27) 6.91 .001
Table 4. Means (SD) and Analysis of Variance Results of Length (by Number of Idea Units) and Quality of Recall Scores
for Narrative Texts by Approach for Year 1
Content
(n = 32)
Strategies
(n = 30)
Basal comprehension
(n = 35)
Participant M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F p
Student % 94.08 (12.08) 74.88 (23.24) 85.86 (21.99) 7. 36 .001
Teacher % 50.36 (25.84) 26.79 (16.74) 47.18 (28.76) 8.35 .000
Table 5. Mean (SD) Percent and Analysis of Variance Results of Words About Text per Stop by Students and Teachers
for Year 1
Note. n = total number of stops across both stories.
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
232
Table 5 also presents the amount of teacher text-
based talk, which showed similar results. In the content
lessons, teachers used a significantly higher propor-
tion (50%) of words about the text than the strategies
teachers did (27%; p = .000). The proportion of teacher
talk about the text in the basal-comprehension lessons
(47%) and the strategies lessons was also significantly
different (p = .000; d = .866). The content and basal-
comprehension lessons were not significantly different
from each other (p = .858; d = .116).
Examination of the second pattern, words per stu-
dent response, is presented in Table 6. The average num-
ber of words that a student spoke during each response
in the discussion differed by approach. Students aver-
aged many more words per response on average in the
content lessons (M = 24.38) than they did in the strate-
gies lessons (M = 11.74) and the basal-comprehension
lessons (M = 13.29; p = .000). The strategies lessons and
basal-comprehension lessons were not significantly dif-
ferent (p = .802; d = .180)
Outcomes Beyond Lesson Texts
In addition to assessments of comprehension of the les-
son texts and an analysis of lesson discourse, two as-
sessments that were independent of the lesson texts
were developed to compare student ability to transfer
knowledge gained in the study lessons. Specifically,
we collected data on student ability to monitor reading
comprehension and student knowledge of and ability
to apply the comprehension strategies stressed in the
strategies approach.
Comprehension Monitoring
Table 7 presents data on the comprehension monitoring
pre- and posttest, which had a total possible score of 12
points. These data were analyzed with a two-way, re-
peated measures ANOVA. The interaction between time
and approach was not significant, F(2, 115) = 0.451,
p = .638, which indicates that the mean change from
pre- to posttest was not significantly different across ap-
proaches. However, there was a significant main effect
of time, F(1, 115) = 9.40, p = .003, meaning that, regard-
less of approach, there was a significant score increase
from pretest (M = 5.01) to posttest (M = 5.87). The main
effect for approach was not significant, F(2, 115) = 0.169,
p = .845, but was of little interest because the change
across approaches was the focus of this analysis.
Strategies Task (Posttest Only)
The purpose of this task was to compare students’ strat-
egies knowledge across the three approaches. This as-
sessment had a total possible score of eight points. The
ANOVA revealed that the scores on these tests were not
significantly different for the three approaches following
the study implementation. These results are presented
in Table 8.
Discussion
The results from the first year showed no differences
on one measure of lesson-text comprehension, the SVT,
but did show differences on the other lesson-text mea-
sure, recall. On this measure, both content students and
basal-comprehension students outperformed strategies
students in terms of length, and content students out-
performed strategies students in terms of overall qual-
ity. These results are not necessarily in conflict, as recall
is a productive measure that may capture a higher level
of comprehension than would a multiple-choice test,
such as the SVT. Even though the differences among
approaches were limited to the recall measure, we
view these results as meaningful because the contrast
Approach n M (SD) F p
Content 32 24.38 (11.99) 15.523 .00
Strategies 30 11.74 (9.18)
Basal
comprehension
35 13.29 (8.14)
Table 6. Means (SD) and Analysis of Variance Results of
Number of Words per Student Response in Narrative
Lessons for Year 1
Note. n = total number of stops across both stories.
Pretest Posttest
Approach n M (SD) M (SD)
Content 43 5.14 (2.61) 5.72 (2.72)
Strategies 37 4.97 (2.57) 6.20 (2.46)
Basal
comprehension
38 4.90 (2.76) 5.71 (2.58)
Marginal 118 5.01 (2.63) 5.87 (2.59)
Table 7. Mean (SD) Comprehension-Monitoring Pre- and
Posttest Scores by Approach for Year 1
Condition n M (SD) F p
Content 40 4.53 (1.32) 1.86 .16
Strategies 39 4.90 (1.62)
Basal
comprehension
37 4.24 (1.50)
Table 8. Mean (SD) Strategies-Knowledge Test Scores
by Approach for Year 1
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
233
between the lessons was restricted, providing a very
stringent test. All three approaches had the same les-
son format of interspersed questions and discussion
during reading, and the lessons were scripted and fol-
lowed with fidelity. Thus, the differences in the lessons
were limited to the type of questions asked at the speci-
fied stops. Viewing the recall results as meaningful also
makes sense in light of findings from the analysis of
lesson discourse, which showed that discussion in the
content approach was more focused on the text and stu-
dents gave longer responses.
The lack of difference on the SVTs may indicate that
all approaches achieved adequate comprehension of the
texts. As noted in the Results section, the means on
the SVTs were fairly high. Moreover, in looking at the
spread of the scores across the stories, 98% of the stu-
dent scores were above 50% and about half of the scores
were above 80%. One interpretation of high scores is
that the test was easy for the students. As for the assess-
ments that tapped abilities beyond the specific lessons,
we found no differences among approaches for either the
comprehension-monitoring task or the strategies task.
For comprehension monitoring, we did find gains from
pretest to posttest across approaches. This gain suggests
that all three instructional approaches had positive ef-
fects on students. However, the fact that this change
occurred for all students regardless of approach is in
contrast to previous findings with a similar task where
we found differences for students in QtA classrooms but
no differences in control classrooms in which instruc-
tion was business as usual (Beck & McKeown, 1998).
As for the strategies task, the lack of difference
seems to suggest that the strategies approach did not
provide students an advantage for applying the spe-
cific strategies independently. It seems that students in
all approaches had equal familiarity with the strategy
terms and how to apply them.
A final question is why results for the basal-
comprehension approach did not differ significantly
from the content approach or the strategies approach.
In trying to create an appropriate comparison approach,
we may have inadvertently made teachers’ use of the
basal program more effective. Our development of these
lessons stems from our observation of each of the six
teachers’ presentations of a story lesson before the study
began. We noted that none of them followed the kind
of lesson we planned to providethat is, reading inter-
spersed with questions about the story and discussion.
Our thinking was that such a format needed to be held
constant in all approaches. Further, we provided teach-
ers in the basal-comprehension approach with a script
that contained only the during-reading questions from
the basal guided comprehension section. Asking teach-
ers to follow our script also alleviated decisions about
other kinds of activities that surround the story in the
basal that could compete with story reading and discus-
sion. Thus, our format may have provided a streamlined
and focused approach to the story lesson.
Year 2
Method
Year 2 of the study was designed to replicate our meth-
odology from Year 1, with the addition of random as-
signment of students to classrooms. We also investigated
the effects of the three instructional approaches with
expository text and explored transfer to texts that stu-
dents read without instructional support. Additionally,
we included a passage-comprehension baseline test, a
pretest in addition to a posttest assessment of strategies
use, and teacher exit interviews.
Participants
Year 2 implementation took place in the same school
with the same six fifth-grade teachers as in Year 1. In
Year 1 of the study, classrooms were intact at the start
of the study implementation and, therefore, random as-
signment of students was not possible. For Year 2, how-
ever, we received permission from the school district
to randomly assign students to classrooms, with the
stipulation that we maintain balance by race and spe-
cial education status (including gifted students) among
classrooms. First, we randomly assigned the minority
students in the district (European American and Asian
American students) to the classrooms. The remaining
students (African American) were then randomly as-
signed to the six classrooms. Classroom assignments
were then reviewed to check that special education stu-
dents were represented equally in the six classrooms.
Given that the six fifth-grade teachers kept their Year
1 instructional-approach assignments for Year 2, stu-
dent assignment to classroom was, in essence, assign-
ment to approach. Total sample size was equal to 116
participants.
To test for equivalence in reading ability across ap-
proaches, we used the same Woodcock-Johnson tests of
decoding (Word Attack and Word Identification) used
in Year 1 of the study. In addition, we assessed compre-
hension ability using the Woodcock-Johnson Passage
Comprehension test. Standard scores were calculated,
and a one-way ANOVA was not significant, indicating
that reading ability and comprehension were not sig-
nificantly different across the six classrooms or three
approaches.
To evaluate whether there were significant differ-
ences among approaches in reading ability, one-way
ANOVAs were done for the three Woodcock-Johnson
tests. There was not a significant difference among
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
234
the three approaches for Word Identification, F(2,
113) = 0.17, p = .844; Word Attack, F(2, 113) = 0.292,
p = .748; or Passage Comprehension, F(2, 113) = 0.41,
p = .665. See Table 9 for the pretest scores for all indi-
vidual measures and the standard score of overall read-
ing ability.
Measures
Expository Lessons
To investigate the effects of the instructional approaches
on the learning of content material, three lessons us-
ing expository texts were developed from the book Bees
Dance and Whales Sing by Margery Facklam (1992). The
topic, animal communication, did not overlap with the
topics students were studying in their science or social
studies classes. These additional lessons were imple-
mented in the classrooms after the completion of the
five narrative lessons.
Additional Measures
Expository Data
For all three expository texts, students were assessed
with the SVT. Internal consistency of the total scores for
the three expository SVTs, as assessed by Cronbach’s
Alpha, was equal to .57. Oral recalls were also collected
for two of the three expository texts. To assess learning
of specific scientific concepts in the texts after students
completed their recall, three knowledge-probe questions
were presented (e.g., “What are infrasonic sounds?”).
To develop a scoring scheme, we listed the major
text concepts that addressed each question, assigning
each one point. Then for each major text concept, sup-
porting ideas were listed, and each was assigned one
half point. The total possible score was 17.75, which was
far from what we anticipated students would obtain. But
we needed to include every possible detail that students
might mention. Answers to probed questions were
scored by one team member, and 20% were rescored by
a second team member to determine inter-rater reliabil-
ity. Differences for the 20% were resolved through con-
sensus. For the chapter “Barks, Chirps, and Melodies,
85% agreement was reached, and for “Messages by the
Mile,” 85% agreement was reached.
Transfer Task
One new goal for Year 2 was to assess transfer effects.
Successful transfer involves being able to recognize ele-
ments in a new situation that are similar to those from
the learning situation, something that young and less
able learners find difficult to do (Brown, 1982a; Brown
& Campione, 1981; Rumelhart & Norman, 1981). Thus,
for young learners, transfer of learning may be unlikely
unless there is explicit instruction aimed at transfer
(Brown et al., 1983). To facilitate students’ abilities to
transfer the effects of the instructional approaches, we
designed a sequence of five lessons about such topics
as lighthouses, the Chicago fire, and the War of the
Worlds” radio broadcast. The first four lessons were de-
signed to scaffold students to transfer what they had
learned from teacher-led text discussions to building
comprehension on their own. The sequence of lessons
allowed for gradual release of responsibility to students
by fading teacher-led discussion prompts and increas-
ing prompts for silent thinking. The final lesson was
used to assess transfer through individual oral recall
after the final text was read silently. The number and
type of prompt for each lesson in the sequence is shown
in Table 10.
The prompts followed the format of the instruction-
al approach being implemented in the classroom:
In the content approach, the teacher reminded
students that they were trying to understand what
they had just read and prompted them to describe
what they were thinking about.
In the strategies approach, students were asked
to choose a strategy that would fit best at each
stopping point, followed by discussion of how the
strategy had been applied.
In the basal-comprehension approach, the teacher
prompted students to think about a strategy that
could be used at each stop or a question that could
Approach
Word Identification test
M (SD)
Word Attack test
M (SD)
Passage Comprehension test
M (SD)
Standard score
M (SD)
Content 5.0 (1.4) 6.0 (2.8) 4.2 (2.0) 0.05 (2.26)
Strategies 5.2 (2.3) 5.7 (3.6) 4.4 (2.0) 0.06 (2.96)
Basal comprehension 5.1 (1.7) 6.3 (3.7) 4.0 (1.3) 0.01 (2.47)
Table 9. Mean (SD) Scores for Three Baseline Assessments and Overall Standard Scores for Each Approach
for Year 2 Students
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
235
be asked. This was followed by a discussion of
how to apply the strategy or answer the question.
Prompts for silent thinking were similar, but inter-
actions did not include oral responses or discussion.
Students were given time to consider how they would
respond and then reading resumed.
At the end of each lesson in the sequence, students
were reminded that the kind of thinking they had done
during the lessons should be done when reading on
their own. For all of the lessons, students were allowed
to refer to a reminder sheet designed by the researchers
to prompt students to use the instructional approach
used in their classroom. The transfer lessons were pre-
sented by the classroom teachers toward the end of the
study and were spread over a two-week period.
Strategies Task
For Year 2, revisions to the strategies task were made
to items that showed floor and ceiling effects, and the
four texts with the strongest sets of items were identi-
fied. We judged strong items to be those that had either
performed well in Year 1 or had been easy to revise, that
is, we had been able to hypothesize why a particular
item had been problematic and could readily overcome
the problem.
The other change from Year 1 was that we admin-
istered the strategies task in a pre- and posttest design.
Each classroom administered two of the stories for the
pretest and the other two as the posttest, with the sto-
ries counterbalanced within approach.
Exit Interviews
At the conclusion of the study, teachers were inter-
viewed to determine their overall satisfaction with their
prescribed instructional approach. To ensure honest
responses, we hired an individual experienced in con-
ducting interviews who was not associated with the
study. All teachers were told that their names would not
be connected to their comments. The interviewer used
a protocol of eight questions that encouraged teachers
to talk about their overall experience while implement-
ing the study. The interviews were audiotaped and then
transcribed, and each teacher’s comments were blinded
to ensure anonymity.
Results
As in Year 1, we collected data in two areas: lesson com-
prehension and comprehension beyond the lessons.
Questions of interest in the second year included three
questions addressed to lesson comprehension:
1. Were differences in comprehension of narratives
found in Year 1 among approaches replicated?
2. Did patterns in the lesson discourse help to ex-
plain any differences found?
3. Were there differences in comprehension among
approaches on the expository texts and in the
learning of expository material?
Three further questions related to outcomes beyond the
lesson textsWere there differences among approaches
on any of the following:
4. The comprehension-monitoring task?
5. The transfer task?
6. The strategy task?
All data, with the exception of the comprehension-
monitoring task and the strategies task, were analyzed
with one-way ANOVAs, with Tukey’s HSD tests com-
puted for post-hoc differences. The comprehension-
monitoring data and strategies-task data were analyzed
with two-way repeated measures ANOVAs, with ap-
proach as the between-subjects variable and test time
(pre or post) as the within-subjects variable.
Lesson-Text Comprehension Measures
As in Year 1, two comprehension assessments were used
for lesson texts: SVT and story recall. The SVT was ad-
ministered after each of the five narrative lessons and
three expository lessons. Story recalls were collected for
two of the narrative lessons and two of the expository
lessons.
SVT Comprehension Assessment
SVT scores were averaged across tests for the five narra-
tives and separately for the three expository texts. The
scores were analyzed with one-way ANOVAs for four
different measures: percent correct for the total score,
GIs, LIs, and PPs. The average scores were not sig-
nificantly different among approaches for any of these
measures. The results of these analyses are presented in
Table 11 for the narrative lessons and Table 12 for the
expository lessons.
Lesson
# of oral-discussion
prompts
# of silent-thinking
prompts
1 5 0
2 3 2
3 2 3
4 0 5
5 0 5
Table 10. Type of Prompt in Transfer Lessons
Note. Lesson 5 was assessed for transfer.
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
236
Narrative Recall
The results of the analyses from the oral recalls for the
narrative texts are presented in Table 13. The results from
the length analysis (by number of content units recalled)
for Year 2 showed overall significance for the narrative
texts. Post hoc tests indicate that, as in Year 1, the mean
length of the content recalls (M = 17.32) was significantly
higher than was the mean length of the strategies recalls
(M = 11.77; p = .005). The mean length of basal-com-
prehension students’ recalls (M = 15.63) was not signifi-
cantly different from the content recalls (p = .59; d = .20)
or the strategies recalls (p = .08; d = .66).
Analysis of the data for quality revealed a similar
pattern as that seen for length. The mean quality score
for the students in the content approach (M = 33.76) was
significantly higher than was the mean quality score
for the students in the strategies approach (M = 24.79;
p = .01). The basal-comprehension students’ mean score
(M = 31.15) was not significantly different from the con-
tent (p = .66; d = .19) or strategies (p = .10; d = .59) mean
scores.
Expository Recall
We collected oral recalls for two expository texts that,
just as for narratives, were scored in terms of length and
quality. These data are presented in Table 14. Although
the mean length and quality scores for the content
group were slightly larger, differences were not signifi-
cant. However, there were differences in the scores for
the knowledge probes, which were direct questions
that queried specific scientific content. These data are
also presented in Table 14. As noted earlier, the total
possible score was 17.75 points, which accounts for ev-
ery possible detail about the probed content. The low
scores need to be considered in light of the inflated
maximum.
The average score on the probes of the content group
(M = 4.27) was significantly higher than was the average
score of the strategies group (M = 3.15; p = .02). The aver-
age score of the basal-comprehension group (M = 4.30)
was also significantly higher than was the average of
the strategies group (p = .02). The average scores of the
content group and the basal-comprehension group were
not significantly different (p = .99; d = .02).
Lesson Discourse
As in Year 1, the transcripts of the lessons assessed with
oral recalls were analyzed. The two expository lessons
for which recalls were collected were added to the anal-
ysis, for a total of four lessons. This analysis involved
Content
(n = 41)
Strategies
(n = 38)
Basal comprehension
(n = 37)
Score type M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F p d
a
Total % 77.17 (8.72) 76.02 (11.34) 76.66 (7.37) 0.15 .86 .103
Global inference % 76.72 (9.21) 75.21 (11.18) 76.53 (7.65) 0.29 .75 .143
Local inference % 76.22 (9.86) 76.71 (11.03) 75.12 (7.78) 0.27 .77 .138
Paraphrase % 77.57 (10.15) 75.92 (13.45) 78.09 (8.60) 0.41 .67 .170
Table 11. Means (SD) and Analysis of Variance Results for Percent of Sentence Verification Technique Items Correct
for Narrative Texts by Approach for Year 2
a
d = Cohen’s d standardized effect size, calculated as the difference in group means divided by the pooled standard deviation.
Content
(n = 41)
Strategies
(n = 38)
Basal comprehension
(n = 37)
Score type M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F p d
a
Total % 77.38 (8.83) 75.26 (9.12) 75.08 (7.67) 0.88 .42 .250
Global inference % 75.92 (10.29) 73.83 (10.54) 74.11 (10.40) 0.48 .62 .184
Local inference % 73.07 (10.88) 72.63 (10.35) 71.93 (8.71) 0.13 .88 .096
Paraphrase % 83.46 (10.01) 80.38 (10.44) 80.26 (10.98) 1.19 .31 .290
Table 12. Means (SD) and Analysis of Variance Results for Percent of Sentence Verification Technique Items Correct
for Expository Texts by Approach for Year 2
a
d = Cohen’s d standardized effect size, calculated as the difference in group means divided by the pooled standard deviation.
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
237
examining the same two measures examined in Year
1 (student and teacher words about text and length of
student response). Data for words about the text are pre-
sented in Table 15.
The pattern of results for Year 2 was similar to
the pattern seen in Year 1. As shown in Table 15, the
ANOVA for student talk revealed a significant differ-
ence. Post-hoc tests (Tukey’s HSD) indicated that con-
tent lessons had a significantly higher mean percent of
text-based student comments (97%) than did strategies
lessons (66%; p < .0005) and basal-comprehension les-
sons (84%; p = .002). The basal-comprehension lessons
also had a significantly higher mean percent of text-
based student comments than did the strategies lessons
(p < .0005).
Table 15 also presents the amount of teacher text-
based talk. The content lessons had a significantly high-
er mean percent of teacher talk about the text (42%)
than did the strategies lessons (30%; p = .04) and the
basal-comprehension lessons (27%; p = .01). The basal-
comprehension lessons and the strategies lessons were
not significantly different in terms of the mean percent
of teacher talk about the text (p = .815; d = .183).
The data from analysis of length of student response
are presented in Table 16. The average number of words
per student response in the discussion differed by ap-
proach. Students produced significantly more words per
response on average in the content lessons (M = 26.55)
than they did in the strategies lessons (M = 9.50) or
basal-comprehension lessons (M = 13.84; p < .0005).
Content
(n = 41)
Strategies
(n = 38)
Basal comprehension
(n = 37)
Score type M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F p
Length 17.32 (10.12) 11.77 (5.71) 15.63 (5.98) 5.41 .01
Quality 33.76 (16.85) 24.79 (11.35) 31.15 (10.06) 4.75 .01
Table 13. Means (SD) and Analysis of Variance Results for Length (by Number of Idea Units) and Quality of Recall Scores
for Narrative Texts by Approach for Year 2
Content
(n = 41)
Strategies
(n = 38)
Basal comprehension
(n = 37)
Score type M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F p
Recall length
(number of units)
15.65 (8.60) 12.19 (5.80) 14.22 (5.33) 2.56 .08
Quality 29.19 (14.16) 23.98 (10.34) 27.31 (8.95) 2.08 .13
Probes 4.27 (1.86) 3.15 (1.98) 4.30 (1.53) 5.028 .008
Table 14. Means (SD) and Analysis of Variance Results for Length and Quality of Recall Scores and Probes for Expository
Texts by Approach for Year 2
Content
(n = 56)
Strategies
(n = 52)
Basal comprehension
(n = 54)
Approach M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F p
Student % 97.08 (5.59) 66.05 (26.09) 83.95 (22.83) 32.37 .00
Teacher % 41.83 (32.74) 29.73 (21.25) 26.70 (10.14) 5.43 .005
Table 15. Means (SD) and Analysis of Variance Results for Percent of Words About Text per Stop by Student and Teacher
in Four Lessons
Note. n = total number of stops across both stories.
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
238
The strategies and basal-comprehension lesson means
did not differ significantly (p = .141; d = .59).
Having obtained similar results in Years 1 and 2 for
the oral-recall measures and for the analyses of lesson
discourse, we conducted a further analysis to try to dis-
cern the roots of those differences within the discourse
itself. This analysis was done using transcripts from one
classroom in each of the experimental approaches of the
discussion of Isaac Asimov’s (2005) The Fun They Had.
Below, we considered the initial 163-word text segment,
which features two children, Margie and Tommy, in the
year 2157 who discover an old printed book and are
stunned by it because “the words stand still,in contrast
to the books they read on their television screens. The
text follows:
Margie even wrote about it that night in her diary. On the
page headed May 17, 2157, she wrote, “Today Tommy found
a real book!It was a very old book. Margie’s grandfather
once said that when he was a little boy his grandfather told
him that there was a time when all stories were printed on
paper. They turned the pages, which were yellow and crin-
kly, and it was awfully funny to read words that stood still
instead of moving the way they were supposed toon a
screen, you know. And then, when they turned back to the
page before, it had the same words on it that it had had
when they read it the first time. “Gee,” said Tommy, “what a
waste. When you’re through with the book, you just throw
it away, I guess. Our television screen must have a million
books on it, and it’s good for plenty more. I wouldn’t throw
it away.” (“The Fun They Had,copyright 1957 by Isaac
Asimov, from Isaac Asimov: The Complete Stories Of Vol. I
by Isaac Asimov. Used by permission of Doubleday, a divi-
sion of Random House, Inc.)
In the strategies approach, the discussion for this seg-
ment began with the teacher directing an activity for
monitoring comprehension:
Teacher: Oh, good. Stop there, Honey. This is a
good place to stop and check our under-
standing. What might be confusing here?
Holly.
In response, a student identified a line of text that of-
fered a confusion. When the teacher followed up by
asking what the student could do to help herself under-
stand the confusion, the student replied with a strategic
procedure rather than a way to address that particular
confusion:
Holly: On the page headed May 17, 2157.
Teacher: Oh, 2157. That might be confusing to a
person reading this and what could you
do to help yourself understand 2157?
Student: Ask a question.
The teacher called on another student, who offered a
way to address confusion that is relevant to this par-
ticular confusion, which is about dates, but he framed
it hypothetically:
Kyle: Maybe you could like to tell if it’s a date or
what. Just like if it’s a date, you could um,
see how many years from now it is.
In acknowledging Kyle’s comment, the teacher also took
the opportunity to point out that he had made an in-
ference. After Kyle confirmed doing so, the teacher re-
turned to the original question of possible confusions
in the text:
Teacher: In the future, good. You made an infer-
ence. Did the author once say this is tak-
ing place in the future? Did the author
state that?
Students: No.
Teacher: But yet you knew it, didn’t you, Kyle?
Kyle: Yeah.
Teacher: Based on what you read. And then you
even did some work in your mind, trying
to figure out how far into the future it was
so that was good. Is anyone in the room
confused about anything we read here?
What did you think about that business
with the books having print?
Another student then offered a different potentially
confusing text concept. Again, when the teacher asked
how the student cleared up the confusion, the student
replied procedurally:
Student: I was confused about moving on the tele-
vision screen...
Student: Yeah, that one.
Student: ...the part when it said they turned the
pages, which were yellow and crinkly, and
it wasn’t, it was awfully funny to read the
words that stood still instead of moving
Approach n M (SD) F p
Content 56 26.55 (17.23) 31.134 .00
Strategies 52 9.50 (8.29)
Basal
comprehension
54 13.84 (6.20)
Table 16. Means (SD) and Analysis of Variance Results
for the Number of Words per Student Response in Four
Lessons
Note. n = total number of stops across both stories.
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
239
the way that they were supposed to on a
screen.
Teacher: That was confusing to you, huh? What did
you do to clear up your confusion?
Student: Ask a question, read on, reread.
The teacher asked the student which he chose. In reply-
ing, the student offered a most authentic moment:
Teacher: You could do...which one would you pick?
Student: Ask a question.
Teacher: Yeah. What would you be asking yourself?
Student: What the heck are they talking about?
The teacher responded to the question and then asked
students to make an inference:
Teacher: [Laughs] That’s funny. That’s good. Well, I
think the, the irony of that part is that we
read books where the print is on the pages
and it stands still, don’t we?
Students: Yes.
Teacher: Isn’t that what we are doing now?
Students: Yes.
Teacher: So make an inference about...what these
kids must be thinking in the future?
Students offered responses and then reading resumed:
Student: How weird they were.
Student: How dizzy.
Although the students in this discussion selected im-
portant concepts from the text, for the most part the
concepts are not used for building meaning but rath-
er are treated as instances of how a strategy could be
applied.
In the content approach, the discussion for this seg-
ment began with the teacher posing a question and a
student summarizing the text segment, including key
ideas about how Tommy and Margie’s concept of a book
from their 2157 vantage point differs from the old book
they found.
Teacher: So what’s, what’s this all about? What’s
going on here? What’s going on? Tajae,
what’s going on?
Tajae: Tommy found a book and they’re looking
in it and they’re saying the pages are crin-
kly and stuff and they’re thinking that if
you read the book, you can go back in and
it will be totally different about it but it’s
all still the same and they say that after
you read it one time, you might as well
throw it away cause you’ll, cause if you
read it and you know what it’s about, if
[inaudible] TV one cause if you turn on
the TV and then you watch something,
the next day it won’t be the same page.
The teacher briefly acknowledged the response and
called on another student whose hand was raised who
added other relevant ideas, including the future setting
of the story:
Teacher: Yeah, all that’s very true. Catherine.
Catherine: Its in the future. It says that it’s 2157, so
the book is really old because now, like
in the future, it’s saying that they don’t
read, um, books. They don’t read books.
They read like on television screens and
they’re shocked because the book is re-
ally old and stuff.
The teacher then responded in a way that integrated the
two students’ comments, followed by a third student
who offered elaboration:
Teacher: Yeah, and you, you added to what Tajae
already said. Yeah, this is set in the future.
And the book and the screen are two to-
tally different things.
Student: Um, the words don’t, um, move around
when they’re like...the way, um, they think
they’re, they were supposed to.
In this discussion, students presented ideas from the
text very much in their own language. Even though that
language is a bit rough around the edges, each subse-
quent student contribution seemed to take account of
and build on the prior ones, such that movement to-
ward coherence is evident.
Outcomes Beyond Lesson Texts
As in Year 1, outcomes beyond the lesson texts were
also assessed. In Year 2, this included comprehension
monitoring, a strategies task, and a transfer task. The
strategies task was administered as a pretest/posttest as-
sessment rather than only the posttest used in Year 1.
Comprehension Monitoring
Table 17 presents data for the comprehension monitor-
ing pre- and posttest. As in Year 1, the total possible
score for this assessment was 12 points. These data were
analyzed with a two-way, repeated, measures ANOVA.
The analysis of the Year 2 data revealed a similar
pattern of differences as was seen in Year 1. The inter-
action between time and approach was not significant,
F(2, 113) = 0.830, p = .439, which indicates that the
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
240
mean change from pretest to posttest was not signifi-
cantly different across approaches. However, there was
a significant main effect of time, F(1, 113) = 13.57,
p < .0005, meaning that, regardless of approach, there
was a significant increase from pretest (M = 4.67) to
posttest (M = 5.63). The main effect for approach was
not significant, F(2, 113) = 1.215, p = .301, but was of
little interest as the change across approaches was the
focus of this analysis.
Strategies Task
The strategies data were analyzed with a two-way,
repeated-measures ANOVA. Data for these results are
presented in Table 18. The total possible score for this
assessment was 8 points.
The interaction between test time and approach was
not significant, F(2, 113) = 1.21, p = .302, which indi-
cates that the mean change from pretest to posttest did
not differ by approach. The main effect of time was not
significant, F(1, 113) = 0.340, p = .561, which indicates
that there was not an overall change in mean scores
from pretest to posttest. The main effect for approach
was also not significant, F(1, 113) = 0.540, p = .584.
Transfer Task
The transfer task consisted of individual student oral
recalls collected after students silently read the final text
in a sequence of five passages in which discussion over
the first four texts was faded and then eliminated. The
fifth selection was the transfer text.
Data from the transfer task are presented in Table 19.
The analysis of the length scores revealed a significant
difference in the scores among the approaches. Further
post-hoc analyses were conducted and although the ap-
proaches were not found to be significantly different at
the p = .05 level, there appeared to be a trend of higher
scores in favor of the content group over the strategies
group (p = .054; d = .490). For the quality scores, the
difference among approaches was not significant at the
p = .05 level (p = .055; d = .460), but as seen for length
scores, the score differences again suggested the trend
of higher scores in favor of the content group over the
strategies group.
Exit Interviews
To ascertain teachers’ attitudes toward the lessons, we
hired an experienced interviewer to conduct exit in-
terviews. The interview was rather loosely structured
around three major areas, teachers’ comfort with the
approachor how natural it feltits effect on stu-
dents, and thoughts about the approach in comparison
to other approaches they used or were familiar with.
There were two or three follow-up questions for each
area, with a total of eight questions. Because the con-
versation that each teacher had with the interviewer
was open and free-ranging, all six teachers did not nec-
essarily address each question. But they did all have
responses about each of the three major areas. We
summarize those responses below for the two teachers
in each approach: strategies (S), content (C), and basal
comprehension (B).
“How natural did the approach feel?”
S1 The approach felt very natural to be honest.
S2 It was pretty natural to me because I think we touch those
strategies in every lesson we do.
C1 Not natural at first, but the more I did them I felt a lot
more comfortable. I always wanted to put my two cents
in.
C2 Its not natural to not go deeper. It’s hard to just let them
think on their own and not pull the information from
them.
B1 I really felt sort of restricted. I wasn’t able to give the kids
my background knowledge into the subject that we were
reading or my experiences.
B2 Totally unnatural because I had to stick to that script.
Because Im constantly probing for more information and
more meaning.
As can be noted above, the teachers’ reactions ranged
widely among approaches but was rather consistent
Pretest Posttest
Condition n M (SD) M (SD)
Content 39 4.77 (2.73) 6.07 (2.96)
Strategies 38 4.81 (2.19) 5.88 (2.56)
Basal
comprehension
37 4.42 (2.71) 4.92 (2.91)
Marginal 114 4.67 (2.54) 5.63 (2.84)
Table 17. Mean (SD) Comprehension-Monitoring Pre-
and Posttest Scores by Approach
Pretest Posttest
Approach n M (SD) M (SD)
Content 41 4.24 (1.69) 4.76 (1.92)
Strategies 38 4.87 (1.76) 4.76 (1.95)
Basal
comprehension
37 4.57 (1.71) 4.49 (1.66)
Marginal 116 4.55 (1.72) 4.67 (1.84)
Table 18. Mean (SD) Strategies-Knowledge Pre- and
Posttest Scores by Approach
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
241
within approach. That is, the strategies teachers were
quite satisfied, the content teachers were somewhat sat-
isfied, and the basal-comprehension teachers were not
satisfied as far as the approach feeling natural to them.
It is interesting to note an issue that arose in each ap-
proach regarding why the approach was not completely
natural to them. The teachers referred to their usual ap-
proach in a way that suggests that they often provide a
great deal of information to the students (e.g., “my two
cents”; “give the kids my background knowledge...or
my experiences”) or pose questions in a way that leads
students to the responses (“pull the information from
them”).
“What was the effect on students?”
S1 I think it helped reach out to some kids that were on
the fence, and I think it definitely helped to push them
over. There was still that percentage of kids that seemed
clueless.
S2 I think it helped them a lot. Sometimes maybe the kids
were a little confused, but it helped them with asking
themselves the questions.
C1 Some responded quickly and they were on top of it.
Others you could tell the thought process was taking
place, so they didn’t respond as well. But all in all, I think
they responded pretty well.
C2 It wasn’t just listing facts. They really had to think about
what the author was trying to say. Some of them couldn’t
quite grasp what was being asked of them. They had a
difficult time responding. The students that did respond
did well.
B1 I believe it does help them to grasp the meaning,
however, I believe that you also need to ask other
questions.
B2 The discourse was labored. You’d think they were bored.
I [usually] have those kids with an open notebook, and I
stop and pose a question, and they got to respond, write
something.
As can be noted above, five of the six teachers indi-
cated that they saw some benefit in the approach for at
least some of their students, even though it might have
been difficult for students to respond at first.
“How did the approach compare with others you’ve used or
know about?”
S1 Prior to the strategy study, if we were reading a novel,
I would focus on the story elements. But this was more
meaty. This got them to follow a recipe that will never fail
them.
S2 I thought it was very effective in getting them to engage
in the story and then again to know what to do. I like this.
I liked it a lot.
C1 [In my previous instruction] the children got to view the
story three times. Once on audio, once I read it to them,
and the third time we had the students read the story. I
don’t think that they need all that repetition. They need to
realize that whenever you do the story, whether its one
time or three times, that you need to pay attention at all
times.
C2 The basal has a wider range or variety of questioning.
So the student that likes to stick straight to the point, that
doesn’t understand context deeply, there’s stuff for them.
And then theres also other types of questioning for the
other students.
B1 My approach was close to the basal approach that I used,
except that I found the approach to be limiting somewhat
to the types of questions.
B2 The length of time that’s spent on the story [was
different]. With the basal series, they have the tapes, and
I will often have them listen to the tape before we even
read or partner read.
Two teachers referred to their habitual approach as
doing the story three times,including listening to it
on tape initially. Two teachers referred to the need for
“other types of questions,and the way that one of the
content teachers explained it, it seems that she means
that some students need more literal questions to be
able to respond. The responses seem to indicate that
both strategies teachers and at least one content teacher
would be happy to continue to use aspects of the ap-
proach in their future teaching. Perhaps surprising is
that the teachers using a modified version of their own
basal were the least satisfied.
Discussion
We now summarize the results of Year 2 (a discussion
of all aspects of the study follows). The second year of
Content
(n = 40)
Strategies
(n = 37)
Basal comprehension
(n = 36)
Score type M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F p
Length 16.35 (8.40) 12.41 (7.31) 12.75 (6.16) 3.39 .037
Quality 31.72 (14.51) 25.08 (13.85) 25.75 (10.71) 2.98 .055
Table 19. Means (SD) and Analysis of Variance Results for Transfer-Task Length (by Number of Idea Units) and Quality
Scores by Approach for Year 2
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
242
the study showed a similar pattern as that found in the
first year, despite having random assignment of stu-
dents in the second year. That is, the content approach
showed an advantage over the strategies approach in
recall length and quality for narrative texts, but no dif-
ferences were found for scores on the SVT. Also similar
were the results of the lesson-discourse analysis, with
content discussion being more focused on text than
was strategies discussion and content students provid-
ing longer responses than did both strategies and basal-
comprehension students. The lesson-discourse analysis
included two expository texts that were added for the
second year. Similarities were also found in beyond-
lesson-texts assessments. That is, again, no differences
were found among approaches for the comprehension
monitoring or the strategies task.
The results for the expository texts reflected the pat-
tern for the narrative texts in recall as well as lesson
discourse. Scores for both length and quality of recall
were higher for content than they were for strategies
and about equal to that of the basal-comprehension
group, although these differences were not significant.
Analysis of the knowledge-probe data for the exposi-
tory texts did show significant differences, however, for
the content and basal-comprehension groups over the
strategies group.
A beyond-lesson measure that was added for the
second year of the study, recall of a transfer text, add-
ed to the picture of differences among approaches.
Differences in quality scores and length of recall favored
the content group, with mean scores of the strategies
and basal-comprehension groups being nearly identical.
Although these differences did not reach the p = .05
level of significance, they do seem to confirm an impor-
tant trend throughout the data. That is, any differences
in recall scores favored the content group.
General Discussion
The goal of the present study was to compare the results
of two comprehension instructional approaches (strate-
gies and content) and a control approach (basal compre-
hension) under circumstances that would allow fairly
precise description of what was going on in classrooms
within each approach. To do so, we implemented rig-
orously designed representations of the two approach-
es. The rigorous design included using common texts
within each approach, scripting what the teacher said
and asked students to do, providing teacher training
and feedback, and conducting fidelity checks for every
lesson in each approach. Thus, we attempted to ensure
as much as possible that only the issues of interest were
varied, specifically, how teachers prompted students
to think about text and how teachers responded to stu-
dents’ comments during reading lessons.
Results were remarkably consistent across the two
years, an indication that the lesson design held in-
structional approaches constant over two cohorts of
fifth graders, despite lack of random assignment in the
first year and whether it was the initial or second time
through the lessons for the teachers. At a general level,
our results lead us to conclude that all the instructional
approaches provided for adequate comprehension, and
a small but consistent pattern of differences occurred
that favored the content approach.
In the discussion that follows, we consider what un-
derlies the similarities and differences found, focusing
on the two experimental approaches, content and strat-
egies. We then discuss how the basal-comprehension
approach functioned in this study and finally turn to
limitations of the study and implications for research
and practice.
SVT
Strategies and content did not differ on the SVTs, which
are essentially recognition tests, for both years, and the
findings, as noted earlier, suggest that students in all
approaches were moderately successful (Tables 3 and
11). Our speculation for why there were no differences
is that the format of interspersed oral reading and dis-
cussion that was held constant across approaches was a
powerful influence. Certainly, oral reading of the texts
helped to address decoding difficulties that might have
hindered some students’ understanding had the texts
been read independently. A more interesting theoretical
reason is that the interspersed reading-and-discussion
format might have scaffolded components of text pro-
cessing for students by giving students the opportunity
to focus on important information and make connec-
tions to prior text and relevant outside information.
Thus, for the level of comprehension assessed through
a recognition test, such as the SVT, the substance of
the questions that prompt discussion (e.g., a content
prompt, such as “What’s going on here?,” or a strategies
prompt, such as “What inference can you make here?”)
may be less important than the interspersed reading-
and-discussion format.
Oral Recall
The substance of questions asked during lessons appar-
ently did make a difference for the level of comprehen-
sion required by a recall task. Differences in recall for
narratives favored the content group across both years
in terms of length and quality of recall (Tables 4 and
13). Consider that a recall task requires a constructed
response, in contrast to a recognition task. In construct-
ing a response, a reader has to bring forth information
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
243
from memory, decide which information to use, and put
it into language. A recognition task, on the other hand,
calls for matching information provided with informa-
tion stored in memory. Thus, recall represents a higher
level of comprehension.
An answer as to why the content approach showed
advantage on recall may lie in the nature of the dis-
cussions prompted by the substance of the questions.
Discussions between approaches differed in terms of
what students and teachers talked about and how much
students said (Tables 5, 6, 15, and 16). As our findings
demonstrate, in the content approach, a greater propor-
tion of talk focused on text ideas, and students’ respons-
es were longer. To a large extent, this result suggests that
students do what is asked of them. If questions directly
prompt students to talk about text content, they talk
about text content, and thus remember more text ideas
than they do if questions prompt them to access text
content through strategies. Indeed, strategies prompts
may split focus between talking about strategies and
talking about content.
Lesson Discourse
How the differences found in oral recall played out in
discussion can be gleaned from transcripts of the lesson
discussions in the strategies and content classrooms.
The discourse excerpts that we examined suggested
that the prompts to apply strategies may not lead stu-
dents to consider text content directly but indirectly,
taking them first through the strategy routine. For in-
stance, when asked to summarize, students’ focus may
go to the who, what, when, where structure in contrast
to what’s going on” in this section. Or when asked to
think about a question to ask, students may consider
what makes a good question rather than what is impor-
tant content to be questioned.
The teacher questions asked in the content lessons,
on the other hand, seemed to encourage students to
express and integrate what they had understood from
text, as evidenced in their recalls of the text. Why is
this good? The notion is that the discussion provides a
kind of external model of comprehension, that is, going
through text, selecting what is important, and connect-
ing those ideas to build an understanding.
Expository Text
We had three dependent measures for expository text
that we implemented only in the second year: SVTs, re-
call, and learning probes. As was the case with narra-
tive texts, there were no differences on SVT measures
(Table 12). In contrast to narrative recalls, there were no
differences among the approaches for expository-text
recall (Table 14). The latter may relate to the more dif-
ficult content in expository text in terms of knowledge
about the world. Anticipating such potential difficulty,
we included knowledge prompts, which asked students
to talk about the key concepts in the texts (e.g., “Tell me
about infrasonic sounds”). The idea was that it may be
too difficult to go into memory and retrieve new con-
cepts, but when probes are provided, students may be
able to bring forth relevant information. Thus, while the
potential explanation for no differences on the SVTs is
that students in each approach did moderately well, the
lack of differences in recall may indicate that all stu-
dents had more difficulty because the expository-text
content was unfamiliar. Yet, the prompts seemed to pro-
vide enough support for the content students to access
what they had read relative to the strategies students.
Comprehension-Monitoring Task
The comprehension-monitoring task, in which stu-
dents identified anomalous content, was intended to be
a measure of transfer that might have developed from
experiencing the comprehension approach used in their
classroom. We used this particular task for two reasons:
First, identifying obstacles to comprehension is key to
monitoring one’s comprehension, and second, we had
used a similar task in previous research and had found
differences between instructional groups. Yet the results
of the comprehension-monitoring task in the current
study show no differences among approaches in either
year.
In considering what to make of the comprehension-
monitoring results, two features that also appeared
in both years are of note. First, there was significant
increase on the measure across time regardless of ap-
proach, and second, the scores were very low overall.
The increase in scores suggests that the task did capture
something about comprehension ability and supports
the idea that all approaches had positive effects. Yet the
scores themselves suggest that the task was quite dif-
ficult for students. Our conclusion then is that the task
we used may be of value, but redesign is called for be-
fore it could be a useful measure in further studies.
Strategies Task
Another attempt to assess learning beyond the lesson
text was the strategies task. In the strategies task, we
attempted to measure students’ application of strate-
gies on a paper-and-pencil task. Students read short
texts and answered questions including Which is the
best summary?” and “Which is an inference you could
draw? Results show that there were no differences
across approaches (Tables 8 and 18). This result might
be interpreted to mean that the instruction in the strat-
egies approach did not provide students an advantage
in explicit knowledge of strategies compared with what
they had been exposed to in their regular classroom
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
244
instruction. We cannot draw this conclusion, however,
because the scores were very low overall, and in the
second year, when there was a pre- and posttest, no dif-
ferences were found over time regardless of approach.
We would have expected the strategies approach to
show an advantage. (As can be seen from the example
strategy-lesson excerpt, students did seem to know the
strategies.) The lack of an advantage might be attributed
to the way strategies were measured, requiring some-
thing different from the way students were taught about
strategies. The assessment task required students to read
a short text and then be reflective about “which of four
possibilities...is a good summary...[or] is an inference
you could draw,in contrast to developing a summary or
making an inference through questions and interactions
with the text, teacher, and peers. The task requirements
may have been different enough from what students
were doing in the lesson that students were not able to
transfer their knowledge of strategies to a more analytic
task. Although Kozminsky and Kozminsky (2001) used
a very similar task and found significant differences
among groups of ninth graders of different educational
levels (e.g., academic, vocational, learning disabilities),
those subjects had not engaged in an intervention, and
as such, the task may not be sensitive to instruction.
Transfer Task
Another transfer task included in Year 2 scaffolded
students toward using the comprehension approach
they had experienced in class with a text read with-
out such support. In considering the possible outcomes
of a transfer measure, it is of note that an underlying
motivation for teaching strategies is that strategies are
seen as generative and that learning strategies should
provide students with tools they can use on their own
and apply to any text. We would contend that a content
approach is generative also, as the discussion simulates
the comprehension process, and experience with such
discussions may promote the development of a kind of
mental template of that model that students incorporate
into their own repertoires.
The results for the transfer task showed no signifi-
cant advantage by approach for allowing students to
transfer what they had learned (Table 19). However,
the trend of higher mean scores for the content group
was once again in evidence for both length and quality
of recall and was associated with moderate effect sizes
(d = .490 and d = .460, respectively).
Role of Basal-Comprehension Approach
Why did the basal-comprehension approach deliver re-
sults nearly equal to those of the content approach and in
some cases better than those of the strategies approach?
Our expectation was that the basal-comprehension
approach would produce discussions that ranged fur-
ther from text content than did the experimental ap-
proaches. We based that position on our familiarity
with conventional basal lessons, which tend to target
questions to a variety of issues, including direct inqui-
ries about text content, establishing purpose for read-
ing, character feelings and motivations, connections to
personal experience, applying strategies to text ideas,
and questions beyond guiding comprehension, such as
word recognition, grammar, and progress-monitoring
assessments. Moreover, a host of other activities sur-
rounds the text, such as connections to other content
areas, activities for students at different skill levels, flu-
ency activities, and suggestions for literary analysis, as
well as postreading questions and suggested assessment
strategies. The scripted version of the basal-comprehen-
sion lessons we developed included only questions in-
tended to be asked during reading and only those that
pertained to comprehension. As such, the questions did
not trigger as diverse a discussion as might have oc-
curred had all questions and activities been used.
Thus, the basal-comprehension approach in this
study, which we had originally viewed as conventional
instruction, was quite constrained compared with a
business-as-usual basal control. Constraining the basal-
comprehension approach was done deliberately so we
could understand what was going on in this control ap-
proach. However, we may have inadvertently created
a somewhat advantaged control because the focus on
comprehension might have been stronger than is typical
of basal instruction.
Beyond the scripted and prescribed questions, the
other feature that likely increased the basals effective-
ness was the interspersed reading-and-discussion for-
mat, which we have already acknowledged as a feature
that supported each group. Even though it appears that
the intent of basal publishers is to offer questions to be
used during reading, our observations at the school
used in this study and beyond reveal that teachers rare-
ly conduct the lessons in that way. Rather, they assign
the text to be read silently and then ask questions.
Limitations and Implications
What can we say about our results on a general level
that is meaningful to issues in the field, both in terms
of research and classroom practice? First, the studies
reported here are not meant as definitive answers about
strategies and content instruction. The instantiation
of strategies is only one example of what strategies in-
struction might look like. There are other choices that
could be made in the strategies selected, the way they
were introduced, and the way in which they were ap-
plied in the lessons. There are, as well, other instantia-
tions of content instruction that could be employed. For
example, one might use interpretive questions, such as
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
245
those in Junior Great Books (Dennis & Moldof, 1983),
or global questions meant to draw a variety of student
judgments, such as those in collaborative reasoning
(Anderson et al., 1998).
A question that remains open is whether student
ability or cognitive factors might affect the results.
Future researchers could provide such insight by devel-
oping studies that are designed to consider examination
of treatment by ability interactions.
Another limitation to the work reported here is our
selection of dependent measures. What is measured
naturally governs the effects that are shown, and in any
one effort, there are limits to the number and kind of
effects that are feasible to measure. Specific to the cur-
rent studies, the measurement of transfer was limited.
Clearly it would be desirable to measure further trans-
fer, for example, what students could do if given a text
to read completely on their own. Also desirable would
be to measure such transfer following some lapse of
time after instruction.
Despite the limitations, we see the work as having
implications that are meaningful to research and prac-
tice. We acknowledge that the differences we found that
favor the content approach were small, yet they showed
a consistent pattern. Second, given the very stringent
design approaches in this study, it is not surprising that
the differences we found were small. All three approach-
es used scripted lessons of high-quality instruction; all
teachers were trained, observed, and given feedback;
and each approach was based on interspersed reading
and discussion, which, as noted, provides a theoreti-
cally strong foundation for comprehension. Indeed, that
format may well have been the most positively influen-
tial feature in the instructional design.
An issue for practice is that interspersed reading
and discussion may be a departure from how reading
lessons are typically taught in intermediate grades. It
follows then that our recommendation for practice is
that at least some of the time teachers work through
a text with students, alternating reading with discus-
sion. The value of this practice includes enabling stu-
dents with poor decoding skills to have access to the
text, providing an external model of the comprehension
process, allowing students to have access to their peers’
thinking, assisting students in building meaning that
may be provided by collaboration, and having an expert
other—the teacher—support students’ meaning build-
ing. The expert other also benefits from this format, for
instance, in being able to observe confusion as it occurs
and consider what the source of confusion might be.
The teacher also gains understanding of the complexity
of the comprehension process and students’ individual
differences in engaging in that process. This provides
the teacher a basis for deciding how to intervene to sup-
port students.
Within the reading-and-discussion format, the ma-
jor question that prompted this study arises: Which
better supports comprehension, a strategy focus or a
content focus? Even if results had indicated that strat-
egies and content produced no differences in student
outcomes, there is reason to question the productivity
of strategies instruction if an equally effective approach
allows students to consider text meaning directly rather
than indirectly through strategies. Our results indi-
cate that going directly for meaning is feasible and at
least as effective as pursuing meaning by going through
strategies.
Part of the rationale underlying both content and
strategies approaches is that they encourage active pro-
cessing. But what is active processing? Must it include
conscious and deliberate attention to the process or
just consciousness that a process existsthat active ef-
fort needs to be made toward understanding? Sinatra,
Brown, and Reynolds (2002) highlighted the importance
of an active stance in contrast to explicit knowledge of
strategy application. They explained their choice by
saying that allocation of deliberate attention while read-
ing may not involve deliberate awarenesscompetent
readers don’t consciously tell themselves “pay attention
to what’s important.Further, Sinatra et al. considered
that continued attention to deliberate use of strategies
may undermine comprehension. This is because com-
prehension takes significant mental resources, which
are limited. So if some resources are devoted to calling
up strategies, adequate resources may not be directed
toward the actions needed for comprehension because
students are being asked to do something in addition to
making sense of the text.
Our findings suggest that getting students to ac-
tively build meaning while reading does not necessitate
knowledge of and focus on specific strategies, but, rath-
er it may require attention to text content in ways that
promote attending to important ideas and establishing
connections between them. We acknowledge that the
consensus in the field is that strategies instruction is
useful (Baker, 2002; Block, Gambrell, & Pressley, 2002;
Gersten et al., 2001; NICHD, 2000). But an overall posi-
tive picture does not provide insight into what it is about
strategy instruction that may enhance student reading.
Such a general claim is inadequate for guiding teachers
as to effective ways of using strategies in the classroom.
Our review of the literature demonstrates that how
strategies should be taught is not easily derived from
the research. Among the problems is that strategy labels
do not represent a consistent set of activities, and there
is little description of how students interact with strate-
gies in the course of reading.
It can be argued that the strategies instruction we
implemented was not optimal. Given our design and
iterative review process, however, we are satisfied that
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
246
our lessons were a reasonable representation of strate-
gies instruction. A claim that there are better represen-
tations of strategies instruction carries with it a need
to not only provide such representations but also show
what it is about them that may lead to better outcomes.
Our specifications of instructional procedures and re-
cord of every lesson enable detailed understanding of
what transpired during the lessons and allow reason-
able hypotheses to be made about the learning conse-
quences of the lessons. Additionally, our lesson design
provides a touchstone that can be scrutinized, com-
pared, and evaluated.
So what is the appropriate role of comprehension
strategies in the classroom? Surely, no one would want
to omit from the curriculum the ability to develop sum-
maries or be aware of the need to draw inferences. Our
recommendation is that such strategies be taught but
that they not lead the process of building understanding
of a text. Rather, we suggest concepts such as summa-
ry, inference, and prediction be introduced to students
with examples using short texts. Then those terms can
be used during discussion to talk about such text inter-
actions as they occur. For example, if a student sum-
marizes a section of text during discussion, the teacher
could comment that the student provided a good sum-
mary, or a teacher might point out that a student had
drawn an inference.
In the present study, lesson transcripts showed that
strategies questions did prompt students to bring key
ideas into the discussion but that students spent as
much time focusing on the strategic actions themselves
as on the content of what they were reading and seemed
less likely to connect the ideas. The importance of mak-
ing connections among ideas is paramount. Focusing on
strategies during reading may leave students less aware
of the overall process of interacting with text, especially
in terms of the need to connect ideas they encounter
and integrate those ideas into a coherent whole. Content
instruction, because it focuses directly on important
ideas and making connections, may be more likely to
support students in building a coherent representation.
Notes
We gratefully acknowledge the Institute for Education Sciences of
the U.S. Department of Education for its support of the research
described in this article. The opinions expressed do not necessarily
reflect those of the institute, and no official endorsement should be
inferred. The authors wish to express thanks to the teachers, stu-
dents, and administrative team whose efforts and cooperation made
this study possible. The authors also thank Timothy Shanahan, who
was instrumental in the conception and design of the study; Carol
Baker, whose reviews of data analyses were indispensable; and Maria
Barron for all of her work in data collection and materials design.
References
Anderson, R.C., & Biddle, W.B. (1975). On asking people ques-
tions about what they are reading. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The
psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 9, pp. 90–132). New
York: Academic.
Anderson, R.C., Chinn, C., Waggoner, M., & Nguyen, K. (1998).
Intellectually stimulating story discussions. In J. Osborn & F.
Lehr (Eds.), Literacy for all: Issues in teaching and learning (pp. 170
186). New York: Guilford.
Anderson, R.C., & Pearson, P.D. (1984). A schema-theoretic view
of basic processes in reading comprehension. In P.D. Pearson, R.
Barr, M.L. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading re-
search (pp. 255–291). New York: Longman.
Anderson, V., & Roit, M. (1993). Planning and implementing collab-
orative strategy instruction for delayed readers in grades 6–10. The
Elementary School Journal, 94(2), 121–137. doi:10.1086/461755
Applebee, A.N., Langer, J.A., Nystrand, M., & Gamoran, A. (2003).
Discussion-based approaches to developing understanding:
Classroom instruction and student performance in middle and
high school English. American Educational Research Journal, 40(3),
685730. doi:10.3102/00028312040003685
Asimov, I. (2005). The fun they had. In I.L. Beck, R.C. Farr, &
D.S. Strickland (Eds.), Trophies: Distant voyages (pp. 584–592).
Chicago: Harcourt.
Baker, L. (2002). Metacognition in comprehension instruction.
In C.C. Block & M. Pressley (Eds.), Comprehension instruction:
Research-based best practices (pp. 77–95). New York: Guilford.
Baron, J. (1985). Rationality and intelligence. Cambridge, England:
Cambridge University Press.
Baumann, J.F., Kame’enui, E.J., & Ash, G.E. (2003). Research on
vocabulary instruction: Voltaire redux. In J. Flood, D. Lapp,
J.R. Squire, & J.M. Jensen (Eds.), Handbook of research on teach-
ing the English language arts (2nd ed., pp. 752–785). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Beck, I.L., & McKeown, M.G. (1981). Developing questions that
promote comprehension: The story map. Language Arts, 58(8),
913 918.
Beck, I.L., & McKeown, M.G. (1998). Transforming knowledge into
tangible resources to support pedagogical change: Final report to
the Spencer Foundation.
Beck, I.L., & McKeown, M.G. (2006). Improving comprehension with
Questioning the Author: A fresh and expanded view of a powerful ap-
proach. New York: Scholastic.
Beck, I.L., McKeown, M.G., Sandora, C., Kucan, L., & Worthy, J.
(1996). Questioning the Author: A yearlong classroom implemen-
tation to engage students with text. The Elementary School Journal,
96(4), 385414. doi:10.1086/461835
Beck, I.L., Omanson, R.C., & McKeown, M.G. (1982). An instruc-
tional redesign of reading lessons: Effects on comprehension.
Reading Research Quarterly, 17(4), 462–481. doi:10.2307/747566
Block, C.C. (1993). Strategy instruction in a literature-based read-
ing program. The Elementary School Journal, 94(2), 139 –151.
doi:10.1086/461756
Block, C.C., Gambrell, L.B., & Pressley, M. (Eds.). (2002). Improving
comprehension instruction: Rethinking research, theory, and classroom
practice. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; Newark, DE: International
Reading Association.
Brown, A.L. (1981). Metacognition and reading and writing: The
development and facilitation of selective attention strategies for
learning from texts. In M.L. Kamil (Ed.), Directions in reading:
Research and instruction (pp. 2143). Washington, DC: National
Reading Conference.
Brown, A.L. (1982a). Learning and development: The problems of
compatibility, access and induction. Human Development, 25(2),
89115.
Brown, A.L. (1982b). Learning how to learn from reading. In J.A.
Langer & M.T. Smith-Burke (Eds.), Reader meets author/bridg-
ing the gap (pp. 26–54). Newark, DE: International Reading
Association.
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
247
Brown, A.L., Bransford, J.D., Ferrara, R.A., & Campione, J.C. (1983).
Learning, remembering, and understanding. In P.H. Mussen
(Series Ed.), J.H. Flavell & E.M. Markman (Vol. Eds.), Handbook
of child psychology: Vol. 3. Cognitive Development (4th ed., pp. 77
166). New York: Wiley.
Brown, A.L., & Campione, J.C. (1981). Inducing flexible thinking:
The problem of access. In M. Friedman, J.P. Das, & N. O’Connor
(Eds.), Intelligence and learning (pp. 515–529). New York: Plenum.
Brown, A.L., & Smiley, S.S. (1977). Rating the importance of struc-
tural units of prose passages: A problem of metacognitive devel-
opment. Child Development, 48(1), 1–8. doi:10.2307/1128873
Brown, A.L., & Smiley, S.S. (1978). The development of strate-
gies for studying texts. Child Development, 49(4), 1076 –1088.
doi:10.2307/1128747
Carnine, D.W., Kame’enui, E.J., & Woolfson, N. (1982). Training
of textual dimensions related to text-based inferences (Brief
Research Report). Journal of Reading Behavior, 14(3), 335–340.
Carver, R.P. (1987). Should reading comprehension skills be taught?
In J.E. Readence & R.S. Baldwin (Eds.), Research in literacy: Merging
perspectives. 36th yearbook of the National Reading Conference (pp.
115–126). Rochester, NY: National Reading Conference.
Chinn, C.A., Anderson, R.C., & Waggoner, M.A. (2001). Patterns of
discourse in two kinds of literature discussion. Reading Research
Quarterly, 36(4), 378411. doi:10.1598/RRQ.36.4.3
Chinn, C.A., O’Donnell, A.M., & Jinks, T.S. (2000). The structure
of discourse in collaborative learning. Journal of Experimental
Education, 69(1), 77–97.
Dennis, R.P. & Moldof, E.P. (1983). A handbook on interpretive reading
and discussion. Chicago: Great Books Foundation.
Dole, J.A., Duffy, G.G., Roehler, L.R., & Pearson, P.D. (1991). Moving
from the old to the new: Research on reading comprehension in-
struction. Review of Educational Research, 61(2), 239–264.
Duffy, G.G., & Roehler, L.R. (1989). Why strategy instruction is so
difficult and what we need to do about it. In C.B. McCormick,
G.E. Miller, & M. Pressley (Eds.), Cognitive strategy research: From
basic research to educational applications (pp. 133–157). New York:
Springer-Verlag.
Duffy, G.G., Roehler, L.R., Sivan, E., Rackliffe, G., Book, C., Meloth,
M.S., et al. (1987). Effects of explaining the reasoning associated
with using reading strategies. Reading Research Quarterly, 22(3),
347–368. doi:10.2307/747973
Durkin, D. (19781979). What classroom observations reveal about
reading comprehension instruction. Reading Research Quarterly,
14(4), 481–533. doi:10.1598/RRQ.14.4.2
Eeds, M., & Wells, D. (1989). Grand conversations: An exploration
of meaning construction in literature study groups. Research in the
Teaching of English, 23(1), 4–29.
Facklam, M. (1992). Bees dance and whales sing. San Francisco: Sierra
Club Books for Children.
Flesch, R. (n.d.). How to write plain English. Retrieved March 17,
2008, from www.mang.canterbury.ac.nz/writing_guide/writing/
flesch.shtml
Gersten, R., Fuchs, L.S., Williams, J.P., & Baker, S. (2001). Teaching
reading comprehension strategies to students with learn-
ing disabilities. Review of Educational Research, 71(2), 279–320.
doi:10.3102/00346543071002279
Graesser, A.C. (2007). An introduction to strategic reading compre-
hension. In D.S. McNamara (Ed.), Reading comprehension strate-
gies: Theories, interventions, and technologies (pp. 3–26). New York:
Erlbaum.
Graesser, A.C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing
inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological
Review, 101(3), 371–395. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.3.371
Graves, M.F., Cooke, C.L., & LaBerge, M.J. (1983). Effects of pre-
viewing difficult short stories on low ability junior high school
students’ comprehension, recall, and attitudes. Reading Research
Quarterly, 18(3), 262–276. doi:10.2307/747388
Great Books Foundation. (1987). An introduction to shared inquiry.
Chicago: Author.
Hood, M. (1981). The effect of previewing on the recall of high school
students. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Minnesota,
Minneapolis.
Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Kintsch, W. (1974). The representation of meaning in memory. Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T.A. (1978). Toward a model of text com-
prehension and production. Psychological Review, 85(5), 363–394.
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.85.5.363
Klingner, J.K., Vaughn, S., & Schumm, J.S. (1998). Collaborative
strategic reading during social studies in heterogeneous fourth-
grade classrooms. The Elementary School Journal, 99(1), 322.
doi:10.1086/461914
Kozminsky, E., & Kozminsky, L. (2001). How do general knowl-
edge and reading strategies ability relate to reading compre-
hension of high school students at different educational levels?
Journal of Research in Reading, 24(2), 187–204. doi:10.1111/1467
-9 817.00141
Kucan, L., & Beck, I.L. (1997). Thinking aloud and reading com-
prehension research: Inquiry, instruction, and social interaction.
Review of Educational Research, 67(3), 271–299.
McGraw-Hill. (2000). Terra Nova test of basic skills (2nd ed.).
Monterey, CA: Author.
McKeown, M.G., Beck, I.L., & Sandora, C.A. (1996). Questioning
the Author: An approach to developing meaningful classroom
discourse. In M.F. Graves, P. van den Broek, & B.M. Taylor (Eds.),
The first R: Every child’s right to read (pp. 97–119). New York:
Teachers College Press.
McKeown, M.G., Beck, I.L., Sinatra, G.M., & Loxterman, J.A.
(1992). The contribution of prior knowledge and coherent text
to comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 27(1), 79 93.
doi:10.2307/747834
Miller, G.E. (1985). The effects of general and specific self-
instruction training on children’s comprehension monitoring
performances during reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 20(5),
616628. doi:10.2307/747947
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000).
Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: An
evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on read-
ing and its implications for reading instruction (NIH Publication No.
00-4769). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Nystrand, M. (with Gamoran, A., Kachur, R., & Prendergast, C.).
(1997). Opening dialogue: Understanding the dynamics of language
and learning in the English classroom. New York: Teachers College
Press.
Nystrand, M., Wu, L.L., Gamoran, A., Zeiser, S., & Long, D.A.
(2003). Questions in time: Investigating the structure and dy-
namics of unfolding classroom discourse. Discourse Processes,
35(2), 135–198. doi:10.1207/S15326950DP3502_3
Omanson, R.C. (1982). An analysis of narratives: Identifying cen-
tral, supportive, and distracting content. Discourse Processes,
5(34), 195224.
Palincsar, A.S., & Brown, A.L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of
comprehension-fostering and comprehension-monitoring activi-
ties. Cognition and Instruction, 1(2), 117175.
Paris, S.G., Cross, D.R., & Lipson, M.Y. (1984). Informed strategies
for learning: A program to improve children’s reading aware-
ness and comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(6),
1239–1252. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.76.6.1239
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
248
Pearson, P.D., & Fielding, L. (1991). Comprehension instruction.
In R. Barr, M.L. Kamil, P.B. Mosenthal, & P.D. Pearson (Eds.),
Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2, pp. 815860). White Plains,
NY: Longman.
Pearson, P.D., Hansen, J., & Gordon, C. (1979). The effect of back-
ground knowledge on young children’s comprehension of ex-
plicit and implicit information. Journal of Reading Behavior, 11(3),
201–209.
Pressley, M., El-Dinary, P.B., Gaskins, I., Schuder, T., Bergman, J.L.,
Almasi, J., et al. (1992). Beyond direct explanation: Transactional
instruction of reading comprehension strategies. The Elementary
School Journal, 92(5), 513–555. doi:10.1086/461705
Rickards, J.P. (1976). Stimulating high-level comprehension by in-
terspersing questions in text passages. Educational Technology,
16(11), 13–17.
Rickards, J.P., & Hatcher, C.W. (1977–1978). Interspersed meaning-
ful learning questions as semantic cues for poor comprehenders.
Reading Research Quarterly, 13(4), 538–553. doi:10.2307/747511
Rinehart, S.D., Stahl, S.A., & Erickson, L.G. (1986). Some effects
of summarization training on reading and studying. Reading
Research Quarterly, 21(4), 422–438. doi:10.2307/747614
Rothkopf, E.Z. (1966). Learning from written instructive materials:
An exploration of the control of inspection behavior by test-like
events. American Educational Research Journal, 3(4), 241–249.
Rothkopf, E.Z. (1972). Structural text features and the control of
processes in learning from written materials. In R.O. Freedle &
J.B. Carroll (Eds.), Language comprehension and the acquisition of
knowledge (pp. 315335). Washington, DC: Winston.
Royer, J.M., Carlo, M.S., Dufresne, R., & Mestre, J. (1996). The as-
sessment of levels of domain expertise while reading. Cognition
and Instruction, 14(3), 373408. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci1403_4
Royer, J.M., Hastings, C.N., & Hook, C. (1979). A sentence verifica-
tion technique for measuring reading comprehension. Journal of
Reading Behavior, 11(4), 355–363.
Rumelhart, D.E., & Norman, D.A. (1981). Analogical processes in
learning. In J.R. Anderson (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisi-
tion (pp. 335360). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Sandora, C., Beck, I.L., & McKeown, M.G. (1999). A comparison
of two discussion strategies on students comprehension and
interpretation of complex literature. Reading Psychology, 20(3),
177–212. doi:10.1080/027027199278394
Saunders, W.M., & Goldenberg, C. (1999). Effects of instruc-
tional conversations and literature logs on limited- and fluent-
English-proficient students’ story comprehension and thematic
understanding. The Elementary School Journal, 99(4), 277–301.
doi:10.1086/461927
Schmitt, M.C. (1988). The effects of an elaborated directed read-
ing activity on the metacomprehension skills of third graders. In
J.E. Readence & R.S. Baldwin (Eds.), Dialogues in literacy research:
Thirty-seventh yearbook of the National Reading Conference (pp. 167–
181). Chicago: National Reading Conference.
Sharp, A.M. (1995). Philosophy for children and the development
of ethical values. Early Child Development and Care, 107(1), 45–55.
doi:10.1080/0300443951070106
Sinatra, G.M., Brown, K.J., & Reynolds, R. (2002). Implications
of cognitive resource allocation for comprehension strategies
instruction. In C.C. Block & M. Pressley (Eds.), Comprehension
instruction: Research-based best practices (pp. 62–76). New York:
Guilford.
Snow, C.E., Burns, M.S., & Griffin P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing
reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National
Academy Press.
Soto, G. (2005). Off and running. In I.L. Beck, R.C. Farr, & D.S.
Strickland (Eds.), Trophies: Distant voyages (pp. 492–506).
Chicago: Harcourt.
Sternberg, R.J. (1979). The nature of mental abilities. American
Psychologist, 34(3), 214–230. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.34.3.214
Sternberg, R.J. (1982). A componential approach to intellectual de-
velopment. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of
human intelligence (Vol. 1, pp. 413463). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Symons, S., Snyder, B.L., Cariglia-Bull, T., & Pressley, M. (1989).
Why be optimistic about cognitive strategy instruction? In C.B.
McCormick, G.E. Miller, & M. Pressley (Eds.), Cognitive strategy
research: From basic research to educational applications (pp. 332).
New York: Springer-Verlag.
Tierney, R.J., & Cunningham, J.W. (1984). Research on teaching
reading comprehension. In P.D. Pearson, R. Barr, M.L. Kamil, &
P.B. Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 609655).
White Plains, NY: Longman.
Trabasso, T., Secco, T., & van den Broek, P.W. (1984). Casual cohe-
sion and story coherence. In H. Mandl, N.L. Stein, & T. Trabasso
(Eds.), Learning and comprehension of text (pp. 83–111). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
van den Broek, P., Young, M., Tzeng, Y., & Linderholm, T. (1998).
The landscape model of reading: Inferences and the on-line con-
struction of a memory representation. In H. van Oostendorp &
S.R. Goldman (Eds.), The construction of mental representations dur-
ing reading (pp. 71–98). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Van Dijk, T.A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse compre-
hension. New York: Academic.
Watts, G.H., & Anderson, R.C. (1971). Effects of three types of in-
serted questions on learning from prose. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 62(5), 387–394. doi:10.1037/h0031633
Wegerif, R., Mercer, N., & Dawes, L. (1999). From social interac-
tion to individual reasoning: An empirical investigation of a pos-
sible socio-cultural model of cognitive development. Learning and
Instruction, 9(6), 493–516. doi:10.1016/S0959-4752(99)00013-4
Wilkinson, I.A.G., Soter, A.O., & Murphy, P.K. (in press). Developing
a model of quality talk about literary text. In M.G. McKeown & L.
Kucan (Eds.), Bringing reading researchers to life: Essays in honor of
Isabel L. Beck. New York: Guilford.
Woodcock, R.W., Mather, N., & Schrank, F.A. (2004). Woodcock-
Johnson III Diagnostic Reading Battery. Itasca, IL: Riverside.
Yost, M., Avila, L., & Vexler, E.B. (1977). Effect on learning of
postinstructional responses to questions of differing degrees of
complexity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69(4), 399–408.
doi:10.1037/0022-0663.69.4.399
Submitted April 8, 2008
Final revision received February 20, 2009
Accepted March 3, 2009
Margaret G. McKeown is a clinical professor of education
and a senior scientist at the Learning Research and
Development Center, University of Pittsburgh, PA, USA;
e-mail mckeown@pitt.edu.
Isabel L. Beck is professor emerita in the School of
Education at the University of Pittsburgh, PA, USA; e-mail
ibeck@pitt.edu.
Ronette G.K. Blake is a research specialist at the Learning
Research and Development Center at the University of
Pittsburgh, PA, USA; e-mail rgk6@pitt.edu.
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
249
Narrative Texts From Basal Series
Frindle by Andrew Clements
This story is classified as realistic fiction. It is one of
the simpler stories in this theme.
Synopsis: The witty class clown invents a new word
for pen and comes up with a plan to get all of his class-
mates to use the word.
Off and Running by Gary Soto
This story is classified as realistic fiction. A number
of Spanish words have been incorporated into the text
of this story.
Synopsis: A girl is running for class president against
the class clown. In her struggle to win, she seeks advice
from a female relative who was formerly mayor of a city
in Mexico.
Little by Little by Jean Little
This story is classified as an autobiography. This sto-
ry is set apart from the others as it invokes background
knowledge about World War I.
Synopsis: Jean Little, the partially blind author, tells
a story about being teased at school for having poor eye-
sight and struggling with math.
Dear Mr. Henshaw by Beverly Cleary
This story is classified as realistic fiction. Instead of
being written in standard narrative form like the other
stories in the theme, this story is written as a series of
journal entries.
Synopsis: A boy who aspires to be an author copes
with anxieties at school and at home by writing in his
journal. He designs an elaborate alarm for his lunch-
box that wins him notoriety at school, and after writing
about the alarm for a school writing contest, he gets a
chance to meet a published author.
The Fun They Had by Isaac Asimov
This story is classified as science fiction. This is the
only science fiction story in this theme, and it requires
more abstract thought than do the other stories.
Synopsis: A boy living in the future finds a book
about school in our time. After sharing the book with
his friend, Margie, she decides that she would rather go
to school with other children and with a human teacher
like in our present time rather than alone and with a
robotic teacher as in her future time.
Expository Texts
Body Language by Margery Facklam
This text describes how a variety of animals use
body language to communicate with each other. It also
describes how we can read this body language and use
it for our own advantage. The concepts addressed in this
text are concrete and familiar to students. Therefore,
this text appears to be the easiest of the three exposi-
tory texts.
Barks, Chirps, and Melodies by Margery Facklam
This text describes how Vervet monkeys make dif-
ferent warning calls for different types of dangers. It
also addresses the different calls that birds use to com-
municate with each other.
Messages by the Mile by Margery Facklam
This text describes how whales and elephants com-
municate through infrasonic messages, sounds that
humans cannot hear but can feel. This text also talks
about scientific research on these sounds. Due to the
abstract nature, this text appears to be the most difficult
of the three expository texts.
Appendix A
Summaries of Narrative and Expository Texts
Used in Lessons
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
250
Off and Running:
Strategies
Prompt 8: Question Generation
1. Let’s think back about the important parts of what
we just read, and ask a question that will make
us think more about those parts. Ask a question
like a teacher would ask on a test. Remember we
should be able to answer these questions from the
text we just read.
One student responds with a question. Accept all
reasonable responses.
2. Is this question getting at the important ideas,
and can we answer it from what we just read?
3. Now let’s think some more about what’s impor-
tant in what we just read and ask another ques-
tion. Remember, we have to be able to answer the
question from what we just read.
One student responds with a question. Accept all
reasonable responses.
4. Is this question getting at the important ideas,
and can we answer it from what we just read?
5. So why do we ask ourselves questions?
[student response]
Yes! So, did our questions make our brains think
more about the important parts of what we’ve
been reading?
6. If students have trouble generating questions
How can we can help ourselves come up with a
question?
[If needed, hold up cheat sheet/card as a clue]
That’s right, think of our question words. Look on
your cheat sheet and say the words with me: who,
what, when, where, why, or how. So, who can use
one of those words to ask a question here?
Call on one student to respond; proceed as in #4.
Possible questions:
What is Miata’s promise if she wins the election?
Content
Prompt 8
To establish that Miata plans to improve school ap-
pearance if she becomes president and to establish that
Miata feels that her ideas are silly after hearing Dona
Carmen:
What’s this all about?
If students don’t mention point above:
Reread:
After listening to the old woman’s story, Miata was
afraid that she had nothing really to offer.
What does this tell us?
Basal
Prompt 10: Make Judgments
Do you agree with Miata’s statement that the things
she wants to do are just little things? Why or why
not?
Possible response: No, cleaning up graffiti, repairing bro-
ken equipment, caring for lawns, and planting flowers
are big jobs for a fifth grader.
The Fun They Had:
Strategies
Prompt 2: Infer
1. Let’s stop here and make an inference about the
teacher.
Students respond with an inference.
2. If students responded with an appropriate inference:
What did you do to make an inference?
If students can’t describe the process
To infer, we put together an idea we just read with
something we read earlier, or know already, to
come up with an idea that’s not written in the text
Appendix B
Example Prompts From Lesson Scripts
(From Off and Running and The Fun They
Had) for Each Approach
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
251
but the author wanted us to think. Sometimes in-
ferring is called reading between the lines.
3. With this in mind, did we make a good inference?
(If needed, use the definition to help students evaluate
the inference, and if it’s not appropriate, to revise it.)
4. If students cannot come up with an appropriate
inference
We need to put together what we just read with
something that we read earlier to make an in-
ference. So we just read that the teacher is me-
chanical—which means it’s a kind of machine.
We read that the teacher can be taken apart and
repaired, and it has a screen where you can see
lessons.
So if we read between the lines, what can we
infer from those two things?
Students respond with an inference.
If students still cant come up with a good inference
Since the teacher is mechanical, can be repaired,
and has a screen where you can see lessons,
we can infer that the teacher is some kind of
computer.
Content
Prompt 2
To clarify that the teacher is not just a machine but some
kind of computer:
So whats the author telling us now about the
teacher?
If students repeat phrases from the text:
That’s what the author says, but what does he mean
by that?
Basal
Prompt 3: Main Idea
Why did Margie hate putting her homework and
tests into the slot most of all?
Possible response: Because it was impersonal, pro-
grammed, and uninteresting; she had to use a punch code,
which meant her grade was calculated immediately.
Note. The script examples are for the three separate instructional
approaches and thus vary in style and format.
Reading Research Quarterly • 44(3)
252
True False
Paraphrase The wolf blows down the first little pig’s
house and then eats him.
The wolf blows down the first little pig’s
house and puts him in a cage.
Local inference The wolf could not blow down the brick
house because the bricks were too heavy.
The wolf could not blow down the brick
house because he was too hungry.
Global inference The third little pig avoids being eaten by the
wolf because he is smarter than the wolf.
The third little pig avoids being eaten by the
wolf because he is stronger than the wolf.
Appendix C
Example of Sentence Verification Technique
Item Design Based on The Three Little Pigs
Rethinking Reading Comprehension Instruction: A Comparison of Instruction for Strategies and Content Approaches
253
Go-Kart
When I was 11 years old, I got in the biggest trouble of
my life. It was all because my father bought me a go-
kart. The go-kart was fire engine red and had a chain-
saw motor that was a screaming terror. My family lived
in a neighborhood with winding dirt roads. Before long
I was blasting through turns, kicking up gravel in the
go-kart.
The roads weren’t the only things that were dirt. So
were the driveways. But one morning, an asphalt truck
pulled up to our house. Soon our dusty, rutted drive
became a perfect, smooth, black ribbon.
A few days later my mom and dad had to go out.
Before they left, my dad reminded me of my promise.
I promised I would never use the go-kart if he wasn’t
around. If I did, no more go-kart.
After my parents left, one of my friends came over.
In no time we were rolling the kart out on the drive.
I figured a little ride wouldn’t hurt. Besides, my dad
would never know.
The gas tank on the go-kart was empty. We kept ex-
tra gas in a giant, 10-gallon gas can. We dragged the full
can across the driveway and lifted it up, but it was too
heavy for us. We ended up pouring one gallon of gas into
the go-kart and about nine gallons onto the driveway.
The next thing I knew, the go-kart was sitting in a
puddle of gooey black muck. And that muck just melted
away as we watched. We just stared at the crater like it
was a science experiment gone very wrong.
I knew I was going to be in big trouble. And as it
turned out later, I was right. But for right now, not only
had I broken my promise about not using the go-kart,
but I’d also messed up our brand-new driveway. I felt so
bad. I just rolled the go-kart back to the garage.
While I waited for my parents to come home, I felt
worse and worse. Finally, they pulled into the drive and
parked right over the hole. They hadn’t noticed it. Was
I lucky!
I remembered hearing that cars can leak gas. Maybe
my dad would believe that was what happened to our
drive. If he believed that, I was saved!
1. What might a teacher ask on a test to see if you
understood important ideas from the story?
a. Why was the asphalt driveway important?
*b. Why will the boy’s parents be upset with
him?
c. What did the kid do with the go-kart after
filling it with gas?
d. Why did the boys play with the go-karts
when parents weren’t home?
2. What would be a good prediction about what
might happen next in the story?
a. The kid’s parents will believe their car caused
the driveway hole, and the kid will only be in
trouble for not telling them the gas can was
empty.
b. The kid will tell Mom that his friend caused
all the trouble.
*c. When Dad moves the car into the garage, hell
start yelling about the driveway.
d. The kid will tell Dad everything, but he won’t
get in trouble because he told the truth.
3. Reading between the lines, what could you infer
from the story?
*a. When gas gets on fresh asphalt, the asphalt
melts away.
b. Some cars have gas tanks that leak.
c. A lot of kids in the neighborhood had go-
karts.
d. You have to take special care of asphalt drive-
ways so they stay in good shape.
4. Which of these is a good summary of the story?
a. A kid broke his promise by filling up the gas
tank on his go-kart, and he made a mess of
his new driveway.
b. A kid wanted to use his go-kart, but every-
thing went wrong because he was out of gas.
c. A kid’s friend talked him into breaking the
rules with his go-kart, and because of that
they messed up the driveway.
*d. A kid broke his promise and tried to use his
go-kart without his dad, and he ruined their
new driveway doing it.
Note. * indicates correct choice.
Appendix D
Example of Strategies-Task Text
and Questions